Verifying Feighner’s Hypothesis; Anorexia Nervosa Is Not a Psychiatric Disorder

Verifying Feighner’s Hypothesis; Anorexia Nervosa Is Not a Psychiatric Disorder

Abstract

Mental causation takes explanatory priority over evolutionary biology in most accounts of eating disorders. The evolutionary threat of starvation has produced a brain that assists us in the search for food and mental change emerges as a consequence. The major mental causation hypothesis: anxiety causes eating disorders, has been extensively tested and falsified. The subsidiary hypothesis: anxiety and eating disorders are caused by the same genotype, generates inconsistent results because the phenotypes are not traits, but vary along dimensions. Challenging the mental causation hypothesis in noted that anorexic patients are physically hyperactive, hoarding for food, and they are rewarded for maintaining a low body weight. In 1996, Feighner’s hypothesis was formalized, relating the patients’ behavioral phenotype to the brain mechanisms of reward and attention (), and in 2002, the hypothesis was clinically verified by training patients how to eat normally, thus improving outcomes (). Seventeen years later we provide evidence supporting Feighner’s hypothesis by demonstrating that in 2012, 20 out of 37 patients who were referred by a psychiatrist, had a psychiatric diagnosis that differed from the diagnosis indicated by the SCID-I. Out of the 174 patients who were admitted in 2012, most through self-referral, there was significant disagreement between the outcomes of the SCID-I interview and the patient’s subjective experience of a psychiatric problem in 110 of the cases. In addition, 358 anorexic patients treated to remission scored high on the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale, but an item response analysis indicated one (unknown) underlying dimension, rather than the three dimensions the scale can dissociate in patients with psychiatric disorders. These results indicate that psychiatric diagnoses, which are reliable and valid in patients with psychiatric disorders, are less well suited for patients with anorexia. The results are in accord with the hypothesis of the present Research Topic, that eating disorders are not always caused by disturbed psychological processes, and support the alternative, clinically relevant hypothesis that the behavioral phenotype of the patients should be addressed directly.

Introduction

Scientific advances often come from uncovering a hitherto unseen aspect of things as a result, not so much of using some new instrument, but rather of looking at objects from a different angle ()

A large population-based twin study in Finland reported that most women diagnosed with anorexia nervosa recovered clinically within 5 years and thereafter progressed toward full recovery (). This interesting study should be revisited, because several reviews of the outcome in anorexia nervosa have concluded the opposite, i.e., although patients may experience symptomatic relief in the short term, remission is uncommon, many patients relapse within a year of discharge, and outcome in the longer term is poor or unknown (; ; ; ; ; ). Hence, the purpose of this Research Topic, to break the impasse, is appropriate. As the introductory quote from Francois Jacob suggests, it may be useful to view anorexia from a different angle rather than relying on the hypothetical cause-effect relationships that underlie standards of care.

A possible explanation for the limited success of presently used standard treatments was provided 47 years ago when Feighner et al. suggested that anorexia is not caused by a mental disorder (), questioning the assumption that underlies standards of care (; ). The mental causation hypothesis is expressed in a position paper from the Academy of for Eating Disorders: “eating disorders are … serious mental illnesses … caused by neurobiological disorder(s) of the brain … Anxiety disorders often predate the onset of [anorexia nervosa]” (). It seems likely that challenging the mental causation hypothesis made Feighner’s hypothesis a “sleeping beauty,” i.e., “a paper whose importance is not recognized for several years after publication” (). We suggest that it is time to wake up Feighner’s hypothesis.

This report will present an updated biological theory of the relationship among the mind, starvation and the brain. Outcomes of tests of the mental causation hypothesis are reviewed and original clinical data are presented that support Feighner’s hypothesis. A 23 years old framework for an improved treatment is then re-launched, including a hypothesis on how the brain mediates among eating behavior and mental symptoms.

Biological Theory

Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution ()

Forty years after Dobzhansky stated the importance of evolution for understanding biology, most scientists agreed, but this perspective has been neglected outside the biological sciences (). And most accounts of anorexia nervosa still do not consider the evolutionary significance of starvation. This is surprising because evolutionary psychology and psychiatry are now long established fields (; ).

Evolution and the Mind

Humans offer mental causes, including intentionality and agency, to explain purposeful behavior (, ), i.e., “In many populations with Western European roots, the prevailing belief is that minds and mental states cause behaviors” (). Over the course of evolution, the psychological process of mental causation, i.e., dualism, developed in parallel with the cognitive systems that are necessary for understanding of the physical properties of our environment (). At the psychological level, the idea that everything has a physical basis [for a discussion of physicalism see ] normally co-exists with dualism [for a discussion of dualism see ], but the two are separable, distinct processes ().

Bloom suggested that “we are born dualists. we naturally believe in an immaterial soul, in spirits …,” and significantly that we are dualists “for clear adaptive reasons” (). Earlier, Darwin raised this possibility by suggesting that “mental powers” emerged over the course of evolution (). Now it has been demonstrated that these dualistic psychological processes have played a significant role in between-group competition and in within-group co-operation and thus in the emergence of shared systems of belief, such as religions; that is to say, dualism and mental causation have facilitated cultural evolution (; ; ). Interestingly, psychological processes of mental causation can emerge after within-group co-operation and engage neural substrates that can be dissociated from the neural substrates engaged in analytical thinking, the two are in competition (; ).

However, mental causation is a feeling or an illusion, rather than an explanation; one is reminded of the behaviorist comment: “we talk about free will and choice when we know about behavior but not its cause” (; ; , , ). Yet, systems of belief, including false beliefs, are often resistant to change even in the face of contradictory scientific evidence, and retrospective examination of the “facts” may be ineffective and can even strengthen the false belief (; ; ). Today, research on “fake news” has disclosed some of the factors involved, including political and economic interests, denialist campaigns, shared values, maintenance of social networks and so forth (; ). Nevertheless critical examination of the available data is essential for progress in science, for the translation of science into clinical practice, and for guiding public health policies ().

Evolution and Starvation, a Brain Designed for the Search for Food

Eating and the search for food have dominated human life in feast and famine alike (). Using an animal model, Epling and Pierce related the increased physical activity that emerges in starvation and the high physical activity of anorexic patients to the search of food (, ). An extensive literature on the evolutionary importance of foraging had accumulated at the time (). This suggestion was novel because it departed from the mental causation hypothesis as recognized by , and today evidence has accumulated that an evolutionary perspective can inform our understanding of anorexia as suggested by Epling and Pierce. Thus, the neural pathways that are activated by the shortage of food assist animals in searching for food, rather than eating, in fact foraging for food can decrease food intake (; ; ). The neuroendocrine cells in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus that are activated by starvation are involved in “shaping (the) behavioral choices” needed for foraging, and these behaviors outcompete most other behavioral choices (; ; ; ). The importance of this behavioral shift is obvious, as starving animals and humans must find food. The role of the brain is permissive, not causal ().

Anorexia Nervosa and the Mind-Body Problem

The observation that reduced food intake provokes increased physical activity was made a hundred years ago in experimental psychology (). Shortly thereafter, it was demonstrated experimentally that food intake decreases as a consequence of an increase in physical activity (). A comprehensive overview of activity-based anorexia, the animal model for anorexia nervosa that emerged from these initial observations was published more recently (). The importance of the model for the present discussion of mental causation lies in the reports of the mental effects in healthy humans who participated in a starvation experiment (). While physical activity was an experimental variable rather than a measure of outcome in that study, the increase in physical activity in the search for food has been documented in hundreds of thousands of people during enforced starvation ().

The humans in the starvation experiment were interviewed not long ago and the mental effects they experienced were recently published (; ; ; ; ). While “food became an obsession for the participants” (), the mental effects came as a surprise:

What I wasn’t expecting was the effect it would have on the mind; the total feeling of, I guess, depression, the total occupation with the idea of food (participant)

Crow interviewed 19 of the 36 subjects long after the experiment and noted that they had all of the mental symptoms that are characteristic of anorexics:

The similarity between these [effects of starvation] and what’s seen in clinical settings treating people with eating disorders is really quite striking. I knew all about this stuff before I really knew very much about what happened in the study just from working on an eating disorders unit ()

It is also the case that the mental/behavioral effects of starvation are reversible with appropriate re-feeding ().

Conclusion

The brain mediates eating-behavior responses to food restriction and has evolved to assist the individual in the search for food. Moreover, healthy humans develop the mental symptoms of anorexia nervosa during food restriction because they are starving, but when the same response is seen in anorexia, humans assume a mental causation of those symptoms by default, rather than assuming that the food restriction is causing that response.

The Mental Causation Hypothesis

Anorexia nervosa is a graphic illustration of the influence of emotions on bodily functions ()

Mental factors have long been thought to cause anorexia, rather than thinking that food deprivation causes the mental problems. Given the complexities of comprehending the concept of whether one of these two factors is causative, it is helpful to analyze the concept of co-morbidity.

Co-morbidity

It is not clear what is cause and what is effect among mental symptoms and the eating disorder and much of the problem with understanding this issue concerns the concept of co-morbidity. However, the analysis of causation is complex () and there are at least two problems.

First, the original idea of co-morbidity was of co-existing, but independent, disorders (). From this perspective, co-existing psychiatric symptoms and disordered eating behaviors are not causally related. Yet, the concept of co-morbidity has led to the clustering of psychiatric symptoms that are not independent (), and the mental causation hypothesis assumes that they are related.

Second, mental disorders are thought to be “risk factors” for anorexia. Two definitions of risk are useful: “the probability of an unwanted event which may or may not occur” and the “cause of an unwanted event which may or may not occur” (). Regarding the first definition, one needs to determine why a mental disorder increases the probability of experiencing an eating disorder. There is limited, if any, information that clarifies this matter [see discussion in ]. We therefore will use the second definition, i.e., risk = cause, to examine the evidence for and against the hypothesis that a mental disorder causes anorexia nervosa, the prototypical eating disorder.

We avoid the distinction between cause, mediator, mechanism, and modulator in psychotherapeutic research (; ). In any analysis, cause differs from mechanism and mediators and modulators are replaced by mechanisms as research progresses.

The OCD Hypothesis

Childhood anxiety represents one important genetically mediated pathway toward the development of anorexia nervosa… ()

Anorexia was once thought to be a symptom of anxiety, rather than a separate disorder [reviewed in ], but it is now thought that an anxiety disorder, specifically obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), causes anorexia. We will examine the attempts at verifying this version of the mental causation hypothesis, because it uncovers some problems related to the methods used to assess this hypothesis.

Self-Reported OCD in Anorexia Nervosa

Retrospective interviews were first used to examine the possibility that anxiety disorders cause anorexia nervosa. In support of that hypothesis, 60% of 68 women with anorexia nervosa recalled having an anxiety disorder, and 90% were thought to have had both childhood overanxious disorder and social anxiety disorder before they had anorexia nervosa (). While these anxiety disorders were considered to be non-specific risk factors for psychopathology later in life, it was suggested that OCD is a specific risk factor for anorexia nervosa, although the presence of OCD before anorexia nervosa was not reported ().

In another study, 42% of 672 patients with eating disorders reported one or more anxiety disorders in childhood and 23% said that they had OCD before they developed anorexia nervosa (). But re-calculation of the data in their study showed that 7% of the patients had OCD before they had anorexia nervosa (). Upon commenting on the re-calculation, the authors stated that at least 14% of the patients had OCD before they had anorexia nervosa and that 36% of the patients in an unpublished study had an anxiety disorder before they had anorexia nervosa (). They then reported that 39.1% of 647 patients with anorexia nervosa had an anxiety disorders and that 94.4% of the patients had the anxiety disorder before they developed anorexia (). But rather than having OCD, that anxiety disorder that preceded anorexia was childhood overanxious disorder, the disorder that previously was thought to be a non-specific risk factor for psychopathologies in adulthood (). It was also pointed out that childhood overanxious disorder is no longer a recognized psychiatric disorder () and the hypothesis that it predates anorexia was subsequently falsified ().

These discrepancies led others to conclude that the OCD hypothesis was not supported by the existing data (; ). It was also pointed out that the causal relationship among anxiety and eating disorders might be the opposite, i.e., that anorexia (or starvation) causes anxiety (). This hypothesis was verified by Keys’ study on starvation (). Yet, a recent review re-affirmed that anxiety disorders “typically” precede anorexia nervosa ().

Registry Studies That Assess the Role of OCD in Eating Disorders

Recalled disorders produce unreliable estimates of the actual experiences in epidemiological studies (), and it should be obvious that self-reported retrospective data make it difficult “to disentangle causes from effect” (). However, the existence of large national data registries in which the dates of onset for different diagnoses are available allows for a more accurate examination of the putative role of OCD in the development of anorexia.

According to the introductory literature review of the first longitudinal registry study as many as 83% of patients with anorexia nervosa can be diagnosed with anxiety disorders, which precede the onset of anorexia nervosa “in most patients” (). But that study subsequently found that 3.5% of 5,065 patients had actually been diagnosed with a specific anxiety disorder before they were diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, and only 1.9% had been diagnosed with OCD before developing anorexia nervosa (). Hence, OCD did not predate anorexia in 98.1% of the patients, precluding the possibility that it was causal in the development of anorexia nervosa. It should also be noted how much lower the documented diagnoses of anxiety and OCD are compared to the self-reported estimates of these disorders.

Despite the evidence, the OCD hypothesis has been maintained by reporting increases in the risk for anorexia among patients diagnosed with OCD compared to healthy subjects in a national registry study. Thus, 0.7% out of 19,069 men and women with OCD had developed anorexia nervosa compared with 0.2% out of 190,690 healthy men and women (). While the relative risk of OCD preceding anorexia was 3.6 times that of those without OCD, it remains the case that 99.3% of OCD patients did not develop anorexia nervosa.

In the same study, 0.6% out of 19,512 men who had an OCD diagnosis also had an anorexia nervosa diagnosis, compared to 0.02% out of 195,120 healthy men. The relative risk of having anorexia nervosa among men with OCD had increased 37 times (). Rather than verifying the hypothesis that OCD causes anorexia, the results actually supported the hypothesis that anorexia causes OCD (), because anorexia nervosa before OCD was several-fold more common that OCD before anorexia, although both cause-effect relationships were uncommon ().

But the increased risk is more apparent than real. To illustrate the problem with these calculations: the probability of winning by drawing a number in a lottery is approximately equal to 0. Drawing ten numbers increases the probability of winning ten times, but the probability remains approximately equal to 0.

Conclusions regarding the increased risk for developing an eating disorder following OCD are further confounded by “… the possibility that subtle eating disorder symptoms were overlooked at initial assessment [of OCD]” (), as had been demonstrated in another longitudinal study (). Also, OCD is a heterogeneous disorder in children (), and diagnosis is not reliable (; ). The recent suggestion that both patients with anorexia and patients with OCD don’t like to make mistakes () stands out as a strained attempt at rescuing the OCD hypothesis in the face of accumulating evidence that argue against the hypothesis. Facing the absence of support from a study on approximately 14,000 children, the OCD hypothesis surrendered just a few months ago, although it was replaced by the equally uncertain hypotheses that childhood overanxious disorder or social anxiety cause anorexia (; ).

Genetics and the OCD Hypothesis

While the history of the heredity of psychiatric disorders goes back several hundred years, the scientific framework for understanding it has remained the same, namely that hereditary factors predispose the individual to mental problems and that environmental factors subsequently cause the disorder (). This is undoubtedly correct, a pioneer of behavioral genetics pointed out long ago that all behavior is the result of an interaction among genes and environment ().

Thus, the OCD hypothesis included the subsidiary hypothesis that OCD and anorexia have the same genetic basis (). However, it was reported that only 0.7% of the patients with OCD develop anorexia nervosa and that “the majority of genetic variance is disorder-specific” (). Paradoxically, a highly significant genetic risk factor for both OCD and anorexia nervosa was then reported in genome-wide-association studies (; ; ). Thus, the subsidiary genetic OCD hypothesis was first falsified and then supported. These contrasting findings may be related to the use of registry data some of the studies versus data from genome-wide association data in the other studies. Note, however, that genome-wide association does not address the question of causation ().

Why the OCD Hypothesis Remains Elusive

One reason why tests of the OCD hypothesis have yielded inconsistent results may be because anxiety disorders as well as eating disorders are not distinct categories but vary along overlapping dimensions (; ; ; ). For example, an anxious person may have problems with sleep, attention, irritability, concentration, muscle tension, continual arousal, generalized worry, or fear. Thus, a multiple regression analysis using specific anxiety dimensions such as these revealed that OCD did not offer better discriminant validity than “general distress” in predicting eating disorders symptom than symptoms of panic and depression, which have overlapping symptoms with anxiety, questioning a specific OCD-eating disorders relationship ().

Because attempts at classifying mental disorders into categories based on Mendelian genetics many years ago were unsuccessful, the existence of these categories was questioned (). This issue was resolved 100 years ago when it was pointed out that with many genes involved, a continuous, normally distributed phenotype should be expected in the population (). Recent analyses confirm the involvement of many genes, perhaps the entire genome, in complex traits, including mental disorders (; ; ), confirming the suggestion that psychiatric disorders vary along various symptomatic dimensions (; ).

Recent research offers mechanistic explanations for the failure to relate genotype to eating disorders diagnoses. It is long known that cells of different genotypes make up mosaics that constitute the basis for display of behavior (). Mosaicism was recently demonstrated within brain neurons engaged in the ontogeny and display of behavior (). This opens for a vast number of gene-neurobiology-environment interactions. Add that behavioral phenotypes can be mediated by different neural networks (), and it will be difficult to verify the subsidiary OCD hypothesis.

The ADHD Hypothesis

it is possible that symptoms of ADHD directly cause [eating disorder] behaviors ()

While the OCD hypothesis has not been tested experimentally, another version of the mental causation hypothesis, the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) hypothesis, has been tested experimentally. Some have argued that research on the ADHD hypothesis has just started (), whereas others assume the causal link between ADHD and anorexia nervosa is firmly established (). Not long ago, it was unlikely to have an ADHD diagnosis and anorexia at the same time because the male:female ratio was 80:20 for ADHD and 3:97 for anorexia nervosa (). That has now changed.

The majority of patients diagnosed with ADHD have difficulty paying attention and controlling their impulses (; ) and women with eating disorders are now diagnosed with ADHD (; ; ; ; ). Binge eating in bulimia nervosa is thought to reflect impulsivity, along with a lack of response inhibition (; ; ) and inattention to the physiology of hunger is thought to play a role in anorexia nervosa (; ).

Testing the ADHD Hypothesis

The interview questions and tasks used for estimating and evaluating attention are related mainly to non-food items, including gambling, card sorting and speed of information processing (; ; ; ; ). While these questions may be valid for a diagnosis of ADHD in other subjects, they may be less valid for diagnosing inattention among patients with anorexia nervosa.

Attention is believed to be under top-down control from the prefrontal cortex (), providing the patients with cognitive control, assisting them in eating only little food. Simultaneously, attenuation of bottom-up control exerted from other brain regions is thought to make the patients inattentive to their starved physiology (; , ). On this framework, one study first noted enhanced activity in the prefrontal cortex in anorexic patients exposed to food items in an fMRI scanner and although bottom-up control was not affected, the patients showed deficiencies in card sorting outside the scanner (; ). How should these results be interpreted?

As discussed above, the brain has evolved to assist us in searching for food. The activity in the relevant brain neurons outcompetes any activity in neurons engaged in alternative behaviors. Considering that shortage of food, foraging for food, and eating have been the main drivers of evolution (; ), it would be surprising if someone who had not eaten enough food for a long time, including anorexic patients, was inattentive to food stimuli. Less surprising, a study found that anorexic patients show enhanced attention to food stimuli (), and when examined with a variety of tests of attention, they showed “superior ability to suppress attention to irrelevant information” (). Thus, anorexic patients may consider gambling, card sorting and cognitive tests of information processing irrelevant. In fact, activity in their prefrontal cortex may assist them in processing relevant information (; ; ).

It is noteworthy that the prefrontal cortex mediates a wide variety of functions, including rational decision making, mentalization, intentionality, and even free will and false beliefs (; ; ; ; ). Even with some compartmentalization within the prefrontal cortex it will be difficult to find a part exclusively devoted to top-down cognitive control of eating.

Genetics and the ADHD Hypothesis

It was recently reported that the genetic correlation among ADHD and anorexia is negative (; ), but that ADHD and binge eating are genetically related (). Given this, it has been suggested that the genetic risk factors for ADHD and eating disorders have not yet been clarified () and the most recent reviews do not mention eating disorders among the disorders that co-exist with ADHD (; ).

Conclusion

The inconsistent outcomes of the many attempts at verifying the mental causation hypothesis have the end result of inadvertently or indirectly supporting Feighner’s hypothesis. Analyses of behavior, cognition and brain function in anorexia nervosa will “make better sense” if they comply with the request for neural plausibility () and if “viewed in the light of evolution” (; ; ). It is surprising that mental disorders, rather that eating behavior, assume priority in most accounts of eating disorders considering that eating behavior has been at the center of animal and human evolution over millennia; chewing is essential for the development of all parts of the head, the brain is not exempt (; ; ). Keeping this in mind, it seems likely that activities in the brain and other parts of the body assist any starving human, including anorexic patients, in paying attention to food cues. It is suggested that the neglect of the evolutionary perspective rather than methodological and conceptual confusions in neuroimaging research () explains the impasse in the field.

Validating Feighner’s Hypothesis

No known medical illnessor other psychiatric disorder, that could account for the anorexia ()

The present analysis generates the hypothesis that the diagnosis of mental illness, which is valid and reliable for patients with mental disorders, is less suitable for patients with eating disorders. We offer the following retrospective review of clinical records in support of that hypothesis.

Reliability and Validity of Psychiatric Diagnoses in Eating Disorders

In 2012, 174 patients were admitted to the Mandometer Clinics, most of them through self-referral although some through physician-referral. Following the diagnostic procedures of the clinic (), 32 were diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, 20 with bulimia nervosa and 122 were diagnosed with an eating disorder not otherwise specified. 37 (21%) of the patients had been referred by a psychiatrist and 20 (54%) of these patients had been diagnosed with a mental disorder, including depression (n = 5), anxiety (n = 5), and ADHD (n = 5).

To examine the reliability and validity of these diagnoses, the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) was administered to all 174 patients by clinical staff trained for the purpose (; ). 120 (69%) of the patients scored high enough to indicate the existence of a psychiatric diagnosis, with anxiety (43.6%) and depression (31.6%) the most common results of the SCID-I interview. However, there was agreement between the diagnosis made by a psychiatrist and the SCID-I results in only 17/37 (46%) cases, indicating that there was actually disagreement, although this effect was not statistically significant (McNemar test, p = 0.26) (; Table 1). Note that although the SCID-I interview appears sensitive, 26/37 patients (70%) scored high, it failed to confirm the diagnosis made by a psychiatrist in 7/20 (35%) of the cases (Table 1).

 

 

 

 

 

When the 174 patients were given the opportunity to see a psychiatrist, 60 patients (34%) opted for psychiatric care. However, there was agreement between the outcome of the SCID-I interview and the patient’s subjective experience of a psychiatric problem in only 64 (37%) of the cases (Table 2). There was significant disagreement between the outcome of the SCID-I interview and the patient’s subjective experience of a psychiatric problem (McNemar test, p < 0.001) ().

TABLE 2

Number of patients with a psychiatric diagnosis indicated by the SCID-I interview and number of patients taking the chance to visit a psychiatrist.

Are Psychiatric Symptoms Among Anorexic Patients Truly Psychiatric Symptoms?

Between 1993 and 2017, 358 patients with anorexia nervosa were treated to remission at the Mandometer Clinics in a median (quartile range) of 18.5 (10–23.2) months. At admission and remission, they completed the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale (CPRS), that has been demonstrated reliable and valid for estimating OCD, anxiety and depression in patients with these disorders (; ; ).

Figure 1 shows that the patients rated the level of these symptoms high at admission and low at remission. Please note that the levels of OCD, anxiety and depression were similar on both occasions and that some patients rated their symptoms low at admission and some rated their symptoms high at remission. The similarities in the ratings of OCD, anxiety and depression are verified by the high correlations (Spearman rank correlation) between these symptoms at admission (Figures 2–4).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is fpsyg-10-02110-g001.jpg

FIGURE 1

Levels of OCD, anxiety (ANX) and depression (DEP) in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa at admission and at remission determined with the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale (CPRS).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is fpsyg-10-02110-g002.jpg

FIGURE 2

Spearman rank correlation [R(s)] among anxiety and OCD in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa determined with the CPRS.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is fpsyg-10-02110-g004.jpg

FIGURE 4

Spearman rank correlation [R(s)] among depression and anxiety in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa determined with the CPRS.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is fpsyg-10-02110-g003.jpg

FIGURE 3

Spearman rank correlation [R(s)] among depression and OCD in 358 patients with anorexia nervosa determined with the CPRS.

The CPRS as used for the diagnosis of eating disorders includes 19 items, some of which are specific for OCD, anxiety and depression and some are overlapping between these three domains (Figure 5). A nonparametric Mokken scalability analysis () showed that all items contributed to just one underlying variable (all scalabilities except one were >0.4). One item (scalability = 0.2) indicated more noice than information. A good contributing item should show a scalability >0.3. The result is not surprising as there is a set of items thought to contribute to more than one domain. A subsequent automated item selection procedure () with the domain overlapping items excluded, should reveal the three intended domains, but the analysis confirmed the first finding of just one underlying domain, with no evidence of any underlying secondary domain. A parametric Rasch model () yielded the same result, one dominating domain with high person – item reliabilities of 0.91 and 0.98, and with two items indicating more noice than information. A principal component analysis of the standardized residuals could not find any additional domain (1st and 2nd contrast <2).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is fpsyg-10-02110-g005.jpg

FIGURE 5

Domain specific and overlapping items for anxiety, OCD and depression in the 19 items of the Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale.

It is noteworthy that at admission, 78 of the patients had been prescribed at least one psychoactive drug, 46 were given an antidepressant, 24 were given an anxiolytic, and 25 were given a neuroleptic, alone or in combination. Their time to remission was 16.7 (10.4–16) months. In our clinics, psychoactive drugs are gradually reduced as much as possible as part of our treatment, and at remission 7 patients were still taking an antidepressant, 7 were taking an anxiolytic, and 12 were taking a neuroleptic. The CPRS ratings for patients taking psychoactive drugs were as high as for the other patients at admission, and as low as the other patients at remission (data not shown separately), indicating that they appeared to make no difference in the outcomes either of the treatment for eating disorders, or in the outcomes for their psychiatric symptoms.

Discussion and Conclusion

These data bring into question whether the diagnosis of psychiatric symptoms, using the procedures that have been proven effective for patients with psychiatric disorders (; ; ) are helpful in treating patients with anorexia nervosa. The domain-specific items of the CPRS failed to dissociate OCD, anxiety and depression in 358 anorexic patients and instead pointed to one domain, thereby raising the question about what the underlying variable might be. Could it be that the single issue that triggers these symptoms is starvation? It is tempting to speculate that the symptoms assessed by the CPRS do not reflect typical symptoms for OCD, anxiety or depression that is seen in anorexic patients. Indeed, the ineffectiveness of psychoactive drugs targeting these symptoms in anorexic patients is well known (). It might be significant in this context that mental symptoms associated with eating disorders are inconsistent over time, identifying some of these symptoms at any particular time may be difficult, as they may be remitting and relapsing over time, rather than running a chronic course (). Equally significant, by practicing how to eat normally, patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders as well remit from psychiatric symptoms when psychoactive drugs prescribed for treating the same symptoms are withdrawn (, ).

It is concluded that the mental symptoms are effects of eating disorders.

Summary

Very few patients with anxiety develop anorexia nervosa but it is common that starving anorexia patients exhibit anxiety. The risk of developing anorexia nervosa among patients with mental disorders has been overestimated by calculating relative ratios among low probabilities. Reports on inattention in patients with eating disorders are based on tests that may be irrelevant for someone who is starving. When relevant tests are used, anorexic patients show increased attention. There are no convincing data relating the genetics of psychiatric disorders with the genetics of anorexia. All the failed attempts at supporting the mental causation hypothesis, including its subsidiary genetic hypothesis, can be interpreted as supporting Feighner’s hypothesis that the starvation of anorexia causes its mental symptoms.

A word of caution seems appropriate. Thus, most of the literature reviewed is descriptive, putting constraints on cause-effect analyses (). While some experiments have tested the hypothesis that cognitive processes maintain the psychopathology of patients with eating disorders, the results are inconclusive (). Yet, it remains possible that a psychopathology turns out to be a cause of anorexia.

Psychiatric diagnostic procedures appear to be better suited for patients with psychiatric disorders than for patients with anorexia nervosa and perhaps other eating disorders as well. If psychiatric illness does not cause eating disorders we need to consider alternatives such as that starvation causes anxiety, depression and obsession. If this is the case, then we can assume that starvation is the critical problem that needs to be addressed. If we look at this as a behavioral issue and take an evolutionary perspective to understanding the behavior, we can develop treatments that may target the problem. For this purpose a theoretical framework is useful.

Framework

A simple, parsimonious hypothesis is that self-starvation is initially rewarding and subsequently controlled by conditioning to previously neutral stimuli ()

noted that anorexic patients are physically hyperactive and that they are rewarded for maintaining a low body weight. In 1996, Feighner’s hypothesis was formalized, relating the patients’ behavioral phenotype to the brain mechanisms of reward and attention (). In the context of the present Research Topic, and in the spirit of Occam’s razor, i.e., leaving what is unimportant in the standards of care for eating disorders without notice (), we would like to re-launch the 23 year old hypothesis cited in the quote. It is actually a framework rather than a hypothesis. Crick and Koch have explained what the term “framework” means in an analysis that we have adapted to understanding eating disorders (; ). For example, a framework should be plausible relative to available evolutionary and neurobiological data ().

It is helpful to consult Guisinger once more. Thus, she stressed the distinction between proximate (physiological) and ultimate (evolutionary) causes of anorexia (). On the present framework, the ultimate cause of anorexia is the lack of food, that has plagued human evolution, and the proximate cause is the associated neuroendocrinology of reward and attention and of foraging for food. However, “proximate cause” equals “mechanism” and mechanisms are distinct from causes as noted in the last paragraph of section “Co-morbidity.”

Today, women (and some men as well) diet to avoid becoming overweight and Guisinger suggested that “psychological factors” might make some individuals more prone to dieting than others (). However, one of the best studied of these psychological factors, restrained eating, is similar to the mental symptoms discussed in this overview in that it is more likely an effect than a cause of disordered eating behavior (; ).

On the hypothesis that eating disorders are behavioral issues that should be understood on an evolutionary perspective, our framework has been translated into clinical practice over 25 years, the theoretical basis, the treatment, and its outcomes have been described in detail elsewhere (, , , , , ). In brief, a reduction in food intake causes release of dopamine in the forebrain terminals of the mesolimbic dopamine neurons concerned with reward, encouraging the patient to continue dieting (). Hence, dieting “causes” anorexia nervosa via dopamine release in the ventral striatal terminals of the neural network of “reward.” The hypothesized rewarding role of dopamine has now been confirmed (). In addition, it was originally hypothesized that because dieting also activates the brainstem noradrenalin neurons concerned with attention, anorexia is maintained by conditioning to the situations which provide reward (). This long established line of research has been confirmed and extended in detail over the years (ibib).

The main intervention, teaching patients how to eat using real time visual feedback on how to eat and how much food to eat during the meal, was recently published in a video (). In addition, the mechanism whereby eating behavior affects mental symptoms (although the nature of these symptoms needs to be determined), has been outlined engaging brainstem relays for chewing as parts of a neural network that connects to limbic forebrain areas, including the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex ().

Hypothesis

You are how you eat (; )

Chewing is at the heart of human evolution, promoting brain development and many aspects of physical and mental health (; ). In addition, chewing exerts an anxiolytic effect (; ). Thermoregulation also played a key role in evolution (), warmth promotes health () and recovery from activity-based anorexia in experimental animals (). In addition, warmth exerts an anxiolytic effect in anorexic patients () and warmth has been used to treat anorexic patients over 145 years (; ; ).

Both chewing and warmth exert their anxiolytic effect by engaging the serotonin neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus in the brainstem that project to the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex (; ). These limbic brain regions are influenced by chewing within 30 min (), the time it takes for warmth to reduce anxiety (). This limbic cortical plasticity may extend to striatal dopamine neurons, which are also essential for mental health (). The hypothesis emerges that chewing and warmth exert their anxiolytic effect by engaging the prefrontal cortex.

Testing the Hypothesis

Transcortical magnetic stimulation and recording of evoked motor potentials in peripheral muscles () combined with chewing and standard methods for examining anxiety and administering heat in the clinic () make testing of the hypothesis feasible.

Conclusion

the underlying assumption that [anorexia nervosa] is always caused by disturbed psychological processes may not always be true (This Research Topic)

The mental causation hypothesis, formalized as the OCD- and ADHD-hypothesis, has not been supported possibly because mental disorders vary along continuous dimensions and cannot be divided into discrete categories. This was realized 100 years ago () and is now re-realized (; ; ; ; ).

Biological hypotheses make sense only if viewed in the light of evolution, the mental causation hypothesis cannot be supported when examined from an evolutionary perspective. The commonly held assumption that individuals with anorexia do not pay attention to their physiological signals is undermined when one takes into account the broader impact of starvation. When appropriate stimuli are used, individuals with anorexia pay more, rather than less, attention to hunger cues. Starvation was the major evolutionary threat and humans have evolved to eat large meals whenever offered the chance, not as a result of lack of response inhibition. The high level of physical activity, the difficulty in obtaining food, and the constant low body weight exemplify the meaning of the term “homeostasis” as this concept emerged from the ideas of Bernard subsequently tested by Cannon (, , ).

Interestingly, it was recently suggested that the genes related to anxiety were adaptively selected for at a crucial period of human development, during human migration “Out-of-Africa” about 100, 000 years ago, and have been maintained in the population (). Furthermore, the same genotype may have offered reproductive advantage over the course of evolution ().

We have pointed out the inconsistencies in the mental causation hypothesis continuously since 1996 and in line with the present Research Topic we have offered an alternative framework and translated it into clinical practice over the years. The main intervention of our treatment is to teach patients to eat normally, because “eating disorders are eating disorders,” not mental disorders. As noted repeatedly in the present account, eating behavior, chewing in particular, has been of major importance in evolution (; ; ), and it is unsurprising that disordered eating is associated with health problems. Interestingly, this framework has been extended to the general hypothesis that inappropriate use of our muscles is a major cause of several health problems ().

The hypothesis offered here is but one of several possible hypotheses that emerges from our framework and aims at establishing the link among eating behavior, the brain, and psychological epiphenomena. Obviously, another question comes to mind: if psychiatric symptoms in patients with mental disorders are different from the “psychiatric symptoms” of anorexic patients, what is the dimension(s) underlying these symptoms in anorexia nervosa?

Personal Insights and Opinions

Normalization of eating behavior was demonstrated to be effective in a pragmatic randomized controlled trial () and treatment outcomes were subsequently reported for 1,428 patients, including all eating disorders diagnoses, from six clinics in four countries (). The outcomes of this treatment was then shown to be better than the outcomes of cognitive behavioral therapy for the treatment of eating disorders, the treatment that is thought to rest on the best evidence (). A recent analysis of the data in the National Quality Registry for the Treatment of Eating Disorders in Sweden found that outcomes of this eating-normalization treatment has been better over several years than the outcomes at the other clinics in Sweden (). The differences in outcomes were not the result of a difference in patient characteristics at admission. In fact, our patients are more severely ill than patients admitted to other clinics ().

The success of our treatment in comparison to that in the published literature supports the tenets upon which it is built/was developed. Clearly, it is time to reconsider the mental causality hypothesis and consider eating behavior and its underlying evolutionary adaptiveness. Once we focus on the effects of starvation on eating behavior, the treatment of anorexia and other eating disorders can be improved. Interestingly, the first step in cognitive behavioral therapy for eating disorders is similar in that “the prescription of a pattern of regular eating (italics in original) is probably the single most effective procedure in the treatment,” which “in the great majority of cases results in a marked … decrease in the level of general psychiatric symptoms” ().

Ethics Statement

The analysis of the data in the clinical files, i.e., the registry of the Mandometer Clinics (point 4), was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of Stockholm. The data has been collected since 1993 and is continuously collected. The patients in the registry did not participate in the analysis. All patients entering treatment are informed verbally and in writing that their data might be used in research and if their data are used in research it will be anonymized. Written consent by the patients is not required for analysis of data collected in registries over long periods of time. The patients are also informed that they can request that their data is not used and that they can leave the treatment any time without giving a reason.

Author Contributions

PS, MZ, and CB collected the data. UB performed the statistical analyses. CB supervised the clinical work since 1993. All authors conceived the idea of the manuscript, analyzed the data, reviewed the several versions of the manuscript, and approved the final version of the manuscript. PS wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Stephan Lewandowsky for providing the supporting information.

Footnotes

Funding. This work was supported by Mando Group AB.

References

  • Adam D. (2013). Mental health: on the spectrum. Nature 496 416–418. [PubMed] []
  • Altman D. G. (1990). Practical Statistics for Medical Research. London: Chapman & Hall. []
  • Alvergne A., Jokela M., Lummaa V. (2010). Personality and reproductive success in a high-fertility human population. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107 11745–11750. 10.1073/pnas.1001752107 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Ammar A. A., Sederholm F., Saito T. R., Scheurink A. J., Johnson A. E., Södersten P. (2000). NPY-leptin: opposing effects on appetitive and consummatory ingestive behavior and sexual behavior. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 278 R1627–R1633. [PubMed] []
  • Anckarsäter H., Hofvander B., Billstedt E., Gillberg I. C., Gillberg C., Wentz E., et al. (2012). The sociocommunicative deficit subgroup in anorexia nervosa: autism spectrum disorders and neurocognition in a community-based, longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 42 1957–1967. 10.1017/S0033291711002881 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Bao A.-M., Swaab D. F. (2011). Sexual differentiation of the human brain: relation to gender identity, sexual orientation and neuropsychiatric disorders. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 32:214–226. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.02.007 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Ben-Tovim D. I., Walker K., Gilchrist P., Freeman R., Kalucy R., Esterman A. (2001). Outcome in patients with eating disorders: a 5-year study. Lancet Lond. Engl. 357 1254–1257. 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)04406-8 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Benzer S. (1971). From the gene to behavior. JAMA 218 1015–1022. [PubMed] []
  • Bergh C., Brodin U., Lindberg G., Södersten P. (2002). Randomized controlled trial of a treatment for anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 9486–9491. [PMC free article] [PubMed] []
  • Bergh C., Callmar M., Danemar S., Hölcke M., Isberg S., Leon M., et al. (2013). Effective treatment of eating disorders: results at multiple sites. Behav. Neurosci. 127 878–889. [PubMed] []
  • Bergh C., Södersten P. (1996). Anorexia nervosa, self-starvation and the reward of stress. Nat. Med. 2 21–22. 10.1038/nm0196-21 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Bleck J. R., DeBate R. D., Olivardia R. (2015). The comorbidity of ADHD and eating disorders in a nationally representative sample. J. Behav. Health Serv. Res. 42 437–451. 10.1007/s11414-014-9422-y [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Bloom P. (2008). This is Your Brain. Available at: (accessed September 3, 2019). []
  • Bloom P. (2012). Religion, morality, evolution. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63 179–199. 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100334 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Boyle E. A., Li Y. I., Pritchard J. K. (2017). An expanded view of complex traits: from polygenic to omnigenic. Cell 169 1177–1186. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.038 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Brainstorm Consortium. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360:eaa8757. 10.1126/science.aap8757 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Brander G., Pérez-Vigil A., Larsson H., Mataix-Cols D. (2016). Systematic review of environmental risk factors for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a proposed roadmap from association to causation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 65 36–62. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.011 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Brooks S. J., O’Daly O., Uher R., Friederich H.-C., Giampietro V., Brammer M., et al. (2012a). Thinking about eating food activates visual cortex with reduced bilateral cerebellar activation in females with anorexia nervosa: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE 7:e34000. 10.1371/journal.pone.0034000 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Brooks S. J., Rask-Andersen M., Benedict C., Schiöth H. B. (2012b). A debate on current eating disorder diagnoses in light of neurobiological findings: is it time for a spectrum model? BMC Psychiatry 12:76. 10.1186/1471-244X-12-76 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Buckner J. D., Silgado J., Lewinsohn P. M. (2010). Delineation of differential temporal relations between specific eating and anxiety disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44 781–787. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.01.014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Bulik C. M., Sullivan P. F., Fear J. L., Joyce P. R. (1997). Eating disorders and antecedent anxiety disorders: a controlled study. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 96 101–107. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09913.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Burnett C. J., Li C., Webber E., Tsaousidou E., Xue S. Y., Brüning J. C., et al. (2016). Hunger-driven motivational state competition. Neuron 92 187–201. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.032 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Capusan A. J., Yao S., Kuja-Halkola R., Bulik C. M., Thornton L. M., Bendtsen P., et al. (2017). Genetic and environmental aspects in the association between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and binge-eating behavior in adults: a twin study. Psychol. Med. 47 2866–2878. 10.1017/S0033291717001416 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Carrera O., Gutiérrez E. (2018). Hyperactivity in anorexia nervosa: to warm or not to warm. That is the question (a translational research one). J. Eat. Disord. 6:4. 10.1186/s40337-018-0190-6 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Case D. (2018). The Minnesota Starvation Experiment: Starving so that Others can be Better Fed. Avaliable at: []
  • Cederlöf M., Thornton L. M., Baker J., Lichtenstein P., Larsson H., Rück C., et al. (2015). Etiological overlap between obsessive-compulsive disorder and anorexia nervosa: a longitudinal cohort, multigenerational family and twin study. World Psychiatry Off. J. World Psychiatr. Assoc. 14 333–338. 10.1002/wps.20251 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Chen Y., Lin Y.-C., Kuo T.-W., Knight Z. A. (2015). Sensory detection of food rapidly modulates arcuate feeding circuits. Cell 160 829–841. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.01.033 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Crick F., Koch C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nat. Neurosci. 6 119–126. [PubMed] []
  • Crow S. (2004). Science and philosophy in the Minnesota experiment. Paper presented at the First International Ancel Keys Symposium on Nutrition and Health, Minneapolis, MA. []
  • Dalley J. W., Everitt B. J., Robbins T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69 680–694. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Demontis D., Walters R. K., Martin J., Mattheisen M., Als T. D., Agerbo E., et al. (2018). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Genet. 51 63–75. 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Denny B. T., Kober H., Wager T. D., Ochsner K. N. (2012). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self- and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24 1742–1752. 10.1162/jocn_a_00233 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Dietrich M. O., Zimmer M. R., Bober J., Horvath T. L. (2015). Hypothalamic Agrp neurons drive stereotypic behaviors beyond feeding. Cell 160 1222–1232. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.024 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Dobzhansky T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am. Biol. Teach. 35 125–129. 10.2307/4444260 [CrossRef] []
  • Donnelly B., Touyz S., Hay P., Burton A., Russell J., Caterson I. (2018). Neuroimaging in bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder: a systematic review. J. Eat. Disord. 6:3. 10.1186/s40337-018-0187-1 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Drill R., Nakash O., DeFife J. A., Westen D. (2015). Assessment of clinical information: comparison of the validity of a structured clinical interview (the SCID) and the clinical diagnostic interview. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 203 459–462. 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000300 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • DuBois F. S. (1949). Compulsion neurosis with cachexia (anorexia nervosa). Am. J. Psychiatry 106 107–115. 10.1176/ajp.106.2.107 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Eckert E. D., Gottesman I. I., Swigart S. E., Casper R. C. (2018). A 54-year follow-up investigation and review of the Minnesota study on human starvation and its relevance to eating disorders. Arch. Psychol. 2 1–19. []
  • Eckert E. D., Halmi K. A., Marchi P., Grove W., Crosby R. (1995). Ten-year follow-up of anorexia nervosa: clinical course and outcome. Psychol. Med. 25 143–156. 10.1017/s0033291700028166 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Ehlers M. R., Todd R. M. (2017). Genesis and maintenance of attentional biases: the role of the locus coeruleus-noradrenaline system. Neural Plast. 2017:6817349. 10.1155/2017/6817349 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Epling W. F., Pierce W. D. (1984). Activity-based anorexia in rats as a function of opportunity to run on activity wheel. Nutr. Behav. 2 37–39. []
  • Epling W. F., Pierce W. D. (1988). Activity-based anorexia: a biobehavioral perspective. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 7 475–485. 10.1002/1098-108x(198807)7:4<475::aid-eat2260070405>3.0.co;2-m [CrossRef] []
  • Esfandiari M., Papapanagiotou V., Diou C., Zandian M., Nolstam J., Södersten P., et al. (2018). Control of eating behavior using a novel feedback system. J. Vis. Exp. 135:e57432. 10.3791/57432 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Fairburn C. G., Cooper Z., Shafran R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 41 509–528. 10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00088-8 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Fairburn C. G., Marcus M. D., Wilson G. T. (1993). “Cognitive-behavioral therapy for binge eating and bulimia nervosa: a comprehensive treatment manual,” in Binge Eating, Nature, Assessment and Treatment, eds Fairburn C. G., Wilson G. T. (New York, NY: Guilford Press; ), 361–404. []
  • Faraone S. V., Larsson H. (2019). Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 24 562–575. 10.1038/s41380-018-0070-0 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Fazio L. K., Brashier N. M., Payne B. K., Marsh E. J. (2015). Knowledge does not protect against illusory truth. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144 993–1002. 10.1037/xge0000098 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Feighner J. P., Robins E., Guze S. B., Woodruff R. A., Winokur G., Munoz R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 26 57–63. [PubMed] []
  • Feinstein A. R. (1970). The pre-therapeutic classification of co-morbidity in chronic disease. J. Chronic Dis. 23 455–468. 10.1016/0021-9681(70)90054-8 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Fineberg N. A., Reghunandanan S., Simpson H. B., Phillips K. A., Richter M. A., Matthews K., et al. (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): practical strategies for pharmacological and somatic treatment in adults. Psychiatry Res. 227 114–125. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • First M. B., Spitzer R. L., Gibbon M., William J. B. W. (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Research Version (SCID-I). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute. []
  • Fisher C. A., Skocic S., Rutherford K. A., Hetrick S. E. (2018). Family therapy approaches for anorexia nervosa. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 10:CD004780. 10.1002/14651858.CD004780.pub3 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Fisher R. A. (1918). The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Trans R Soc Edinb 52 399–433. 10.1017/s0080456800012163 [CrossRef] []
  • Funahashi S., Andreau J. M. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and neural mechanisms of executive function. J. Physiol. Paris 107 471–482. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.05.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Gillan C. M., Fineberg N. A., Robbins T. W. (2017). A trans-diagnostic perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol. Med. 47 1528–1548. 10.1017/S0033291716002786 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Glahn D. C., Nimgaonkar V. L., Raventós H., Contreras J., McIntosh A. M., Thomson P. A., et al. (2019). Rediscovering the value of families for psychiatric genetics research. Mol. Psychiatry 24 523–535. 10.1038/s41380-018-0073-x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Godart N., Berthoz S., Perdereau F., Jeammet P. (2006). Comorbidity of anxiety with eating disorders and OCD. Am. J. Psychiatry 163 326–326. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.326 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Guisinger S. (2003). Adapted to flee famine: adding an evolutionary perspective on anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Rev. 110 745–761. 10.1037/0033-295x.110.4.745 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Gull W. (1874). Anorexia nervosa (apepsia hysterica, anorexia hysterica). Trans. Clin. Soc. Lond. 7 22–28. []
  • Gutierrez E. (2013). A rat in the labyrinth of anorexia nervosa: contributions of the activity-based anorexia rodent model to the understanding of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46 289–301. 10.1002/eat.22095 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Gutiérrez E., Carrera O. (2018). Anorexia nervosa treatments and Occam’s razor. Psychol. Med. 48 1390–1391. 10.1017/s0033291717003944 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Hacker P. M. S. (2010). Human Nature: The Categorial Framework. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. []
  • Hanlon C., Medhin G., Alem A., Araya M., Abdulahi A., Tesfaye M., et al. (2008). Measuring common mental disorders in women in Ethiopia: reliability and construct validity of the comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 43 653–659. 10.1007/s00127-008-0342-0 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Hansson S. O. (2018). Risk. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/risk/ (accessed September 3, 2019). []
  • Heinonen I., Laukkanen J. A. (2018). Effects of heat and cold on health, with special reference to Finnish sauna bathing. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 314 R629–R638. 10.1152/ajpregu.00115.2017 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Hollingworth H. L. (1939). Chewing as a techning of relaxation. Science 90 385–387. 10.1126/science.90.2339.385 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Hotta Y., Benzer S. (1972). Mapping of behaviour in Drosophila mosaics. Nature 240 527–535. 10.1038/240527a0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Huprich S. K., Paggeot A. V., Samuel D. B. (2015). Comparing the Personality Disorder Interview for DSM-IV (PDI-IV) and SCID-II borderline personality disorder scales: an item-response theory analysis. J. Pers. Assess. 97 13–21. 10.1080/00223891.2014.946606 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Hutsebaut J., Feenstra D. J., Kamphuis J. H. (2016). Development and preliminary psychometric evaluation of a brief self-report questionnaire for the assessment of the DSM-5 level of personality functioning scale: the LPFS brief form (LPFS-BF). Personal. Disord. 7 192–197. 10.1037/per0000159 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Insel T., Cuthbert B., Garvey M., Heinssen R., Pine D. S., Quinn K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167 748–751. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Ioakimidis I., Zandian M., Ulbl F., Bergh C., Leon M., Södersten P. (2011). How eating affects mood. Physiol. Behav. 103 290–294. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.01.025 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Jack A. I., Friedman J. P., Boyatzis R. E., Taylor S. N. (2016). Why do you believe in God? relationships between religious belief, analytic thinking, mentalizing and moral concern. PLoS One 11:e0149989. 10.1371/journal.pone.0149989 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Jacob F. (1977). Evolution and tinkering. Science 196 1161–1166. 10.1126/science.860134 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Jacyna S. (2009). The most important of all the organs: darwin on the brain. Brain J. Neurol. 132 3481–3487. 10.1093/brain/awp283 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Jastroch M., Oelkrug R., Keipert S. (2018). Insights into brown adipose tissue evolution and function from non-model organisms. J. Exp. Biol. 221(Pt Suppl. 1), jeb169425. 10.1242/jeb.169425 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Johnson F., Pratt M., Wardle J. (2012). Dietary restraint and self-regulation in eating behavior. Int. J. Obes. 36 665–674. 10.1038/ijo.2011.156 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Jost J. T., van der Linden S., Panagopoulos C., Hardin C. D. (2018). Ideological asymmetries in conformity, desire for shared reality, and the spread of misinformation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 23 77–83. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.01.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Kaisari P., Dourish C. T., Higgs S. (2017). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disordered eating behaviour: a systematic review and a framework for future research. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 53 109–121. 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.03.002 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Kalm L. M., Semba R. D. (2005). They starved so that others be better fed: remembering Ancel Keys and the Minnesota experiment. J. Nutr. 135 1347–1352. 10.1093/jn/135.6.1347 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Kaye W. H., Bulik C., Thornton L., Plotnicov K. (2006). Dr. Kaye and Colleagues reply. Am. J. Psychiatry 163 327–329. []
  • Kaye W. H., Bulik C. M., Thornton L., Barbarich N., Masters K. (2004). Comorbidity of anxiety disorders with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 161 2215–2221. [PubMed] []
  • Kaye W. H., Fudge J. L., Paulus M. (2009). New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 573–584. 10.1038/nrn2682 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Kazdin A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3 1–27. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Ke Q., Ferrara E., Radicchi F., Flammini A. (2015). Defining and identifying sleeping beauties in science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 7426–7431. 10.1073/pnas.1424329112 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Kerr-Gaffney J., Harrison A., Tchanturia K. (2018). Social anxiety in the eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 48 2477–2491. 10.1017/S0033291718000752 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Keski-Rahkonen A., Hoek H. W., Susser E. S., Linna M. S., Sihvola E., Raevuori A., et al. (2007). Epidemiology and course of anorexia nervosa in the community. Am. J. Psychiatry 164 1259–1265. [PubMed] []
  • Keys A., Brozek J., Henschel A., Mickelsen O., Taylor H. L. (1950). The Biology of Human Starvation. Minneapolis, MN: The University of Minnesota Press. []
  • Kita H., Kita T. (2011). Cortical stimulation evokes abnormal responses in the dopamine-depleted rat basal ganglia. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 31 10311–10322. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0915-11.2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Klump K. L., Bulik C. M., Kaye W. H., Treasure J., Tyson E. (2009). Academy for eating disorders position paper: eating disorders are serious mental illnesses. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 42 97–103. 10.1002/eat.20589 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Kumar A., Kothari M., Grigoriadis A., Trulsson M., Svensson P. (2018). Bite or brain: implication of sensorimotor regulation and neuroplasticity in oral rehabilitation procedures. J. Oral Rehabil. 45 323–333. 10.1111/joor.12603 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Lang M., Purzycki B. G., Apicella C. L., Atkinson Q. D., Bolyanatz A., Cohen E., et al. (2019). Moralizing gods, impartiality and religious parochialism across 15 societies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 286:20190202. 10.1098/rspb.2019.0202 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Leisman G., Machado C., Melillo R., Mualem R. (2012). Intentionality and “free-will” from a neurodevelopmental perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:36. 10.3389/fnint.2012.00036 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Levin R. L., Rawana J. S. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and eating disorders across the lifespan: a systematic review of the literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 50 22–36. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Levinson C. A., Zerwas S. C., Brosof L. C., Thornton L. M., Strober M., Pivarunas B., et al. (2018). Associations between dimensions of anorexia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder: an examination of personality and psychological factors in patients with anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. J. Eat. Disord. Assoc. 27 161–172. 10.1002/erv.2635 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Lewandowsky S., Ecker U. K. H., Seifert C. M., Schwarz N., Cook J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest J. Am. Psychol. Soc. 13 106–131. 10.1177/1529100612451018 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Lewandowsky S., Pilditch T. D., Madsen J. K., Oreskes N., Risbey J. S. (2019). Influence and seepage: an evidence-resistant minority can affect public opinion and scientific belief formation. Cognition 188 124–139. 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.011 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Lieberman D. E. (2011). Evolution of the Human Head. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. []
  • Lieberman D. E. (2014). The Story of the Human Body: Evolution, Health and Disease. New York, NY: Random House LLC. []
  • Lowry C. A., Lightman S. L., Nutt D. J. (2009). That warm fuzzy feeling: brain serotonergic neurons and the regulation of emotion. J. Psychopharmacol. Oxf. Engl. 23 392–400. 10.1177/0269881108099956 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Mahy C. E. V., Moses L. J., Pfeifer J. H. (2014). How and where: theory-of-mind in the brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 9 68–81. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Maj M. (2005). “Psychiatric comorbidity”: an artefact of current diagnostic systems? Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 186 182–184. 10.1192/bjp.186.3.182 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Mataix-Cols D., Pertusa A., Leckman J. F. (2007). Issues for DSM-V: how should obsessive-compulsive and related disorders be classified? Am. J. Psychiatry 164 1313–1314. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040568 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Mattila-Evenden M., Svanborg P., Gustavsson P., Asberg M. (1996). Determinants of self-rating and expert rating concordance in psychiatric out-patients, using the affective subscales of the CPRS. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 94 386–396. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb09879.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • McNamara R. A., Willard A. K., Norenzayan A., Henrich J. (2019). Weighing outcome vs. intent across societies: how cultural models of mind shape moral reasoning. Cognition 182 95–108. 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.008 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Meier S. M., Bulik C. M., Thornton L. M., Mattheisen M., Mortensen P. B., Petersen L. (2015). Diagnosed anxiety disorders and the risk of subsequent anorexia nervosa: a Danish population register study. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. J. Eat. Disord. Assoc. 23 524–530. 10.1002/erv.2402 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Micali N., Hilton K., Nakatani E., Natatani E., Heyman I., Turner C., et al. (2011). Is childhood OCD a risk factor for eating disorders later in life? A longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 41 2507–2513. 10.1017/S003329171100078X [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Micali N., Solmi F., Horton N. J., Crosby R. D., Eddy K. T., Calzo J. P., et al. (2015). Adolescent eating disorders predict psychiatric, high-risk behaviors and weight outcomes in young adulthood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54 652–659.e1. 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Nazar B. P., Bernardes C., Peachey G., Sergeant J., Mattos P., Treasure J. (2016). The risk of eating disorders comorbid with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49 1045–1057. 10.1002/eat.22643 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Neimeijer R. A. M., Roefs A., de Jong P. J. (2017). Heightened attentional capture by visual food stimuli in anorexia nervosa. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126 805–811. 10.1037/abn0000275 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Nesse R. M. (2019). Good Reasons for Bad Feelings. London: Allen Lane. []
  • Norenzayan A., Shariff A. F., Gervais W. M., Willard A. K., McNamara R. A., Slingerland E., et al. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e1. 10.1017/S0140525X14001356 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Nusbaum M. P., Blitz D. M., Marder E. (2017). Functional consequences of neuropeptide and small-molecule co-transmission. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18 389–403. 10.1038/nrn.2017.56 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • O’Brien K. M., Vincent N. K. (2003). Psychiatric comorbidity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: nature, prevalence, and causal relationships. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23 57–74. 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00201-5 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Oltra-Cucarella J., Rojo Moreno L., Arribas Sáiz P., Sanguesa García C., Latorre Paniagua P., Hidalgo Muñxoz E., et al. (2015). Impaired executive functioning influences verbal memory in anorexia nervosa. Eat. Behav. 16 47–53. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.013 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Paquola A. C. M., Erwin J. A., Gage F. H. (2017). Insights into the role of somatic mosaicism in the brain. Curr. Opin. Syst. Biol. 1 90–94. 10.1016/j.coisb.2016.12.004 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Pearl J. (2019). The Book of Why, the New Science of Cause and Effect. London: Penguin Books. []
  • Pearson C. M., Wonderlich S. A., Smith G. T. (2015). A risk and maintenance model for bulimia nervosa: from impulsive action to compulsive behavior. Psychol. Rev. 122 516–535. 10.1037/a0039268 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Petrovich G. D. (2013). Forebrain networks and the control of feeding by environmental learned cues. Physiol. Behav. 121 10–18. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.024 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Porter T. M. (2018). Genetics in the Madhouse: The Unknown History of Human Heredity. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. []
  • Purzycki B. G., Apicella C., Atkinson Q. D., Cohen E., McNamara R. A., Willard A. K., et al. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature 530 327–330. 10.1038/nature16980 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Raney T. J., Thornton L. M., Berrettini W., Brandt H., Crawford S., Fichter M. M., et al. (2008). Influence of overanxious disorder of childhood on the expression of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41 326–332. 10.1002/eat.20508 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Regier D. A., Myers J. K., Kramer M., Robins L. N., Blazer D. G., Hough R. L., et al. (1984). The NIMH epidemiologic catchment area program. Historical context, major objectives, and study population characteristics. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 41 934–941. [PubMed] []
  • Richard J. M., Berridge K. C. (2013). Prefrontal cortex modulates desire and dread generated by nucleus accumbens glutamate disruption. Biol. Psychiatry 73 360–370. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Richter C. P. (1922). A behavioristic study of the activity of the rat. J. Comp. Psychol. Monogr. 1 1–55. []
  • Robinson H. (2017). Dualism. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2017/entries/dualism (accessed September 3, 2019). []
  • Rösler M., Retz W., Thome J., Schneider M., Stieglitz R.-D., Falkai P. (2006). Psychopathological rating scales for diagnostic use in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 256(Suppl. 1), i3–i11. 10.1007/s00406-006-1001-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Rück C., Larsson K. J., Lind K., Perez-Vigil A., Isomura K., Sariaslan A., et al. (2015). Validity and reliability of chronic tic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder diagnoses in the Swedish National Patient Register. BMJ Open 5:e007520. 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007520 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Russell G. (1979). Bulimia nervosa: an ominous variant of anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 9 429–448. 10.1017/s0033291700031974 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Sanders N., Smeets P. A. M., van Elburg A. A., Danner U. N., van Meer F., Hoek H. W., et al. (2015). Altered food-cue processing in chronically ill and recovered women with anorexia nervosa. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:46. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00046 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Sato D. X., Kawata M. (2018). Positive and balancing selection on SLC18A1 gene associated with psychiatric disorders and human-unique personality traits. Evol. Lett. 2 499–510. 10.1002/evl3.81 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Schaumberg K., Zerwas S., Goodman E., Yilmaz Z., Bulik C. M., Micali N. (2019). Anxiety disorder symptoms at age 10 predict eating disorder symptoms and diagnoses in adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60 686–696. 10.1111/jcpp.12984 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Schneider J. E., Wise J. D., Benton N. A., Brozek J. M., Keen-Rhinehart E. (2013). When do we eat? Ingestive behavior, survival, and reproductive success. Horm. Behav. 64 702–728. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.07.005 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Seitz J., Kahraman-Lanzerath B., Legenbauer T., Sarrar L., Herpertz S., Salbach-Andrae H., et al. (2013). The role of impulsivity, inattention and comorbid ADHD in patients with bulimia nervosa. PLoS One 8:e63891. 10.1371/journal.pone.0063891 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Sijtsma K., Molenaar I. W. (2002). Introduction to Nonparametric Item Response Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing. []
  • Skinner B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York, NY: D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc. []
  • Skinner B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5 1–10. []
  • Smith T. M. (2018). The Tales Teeth Tell. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. []
  • Södersten P., Bergh C. (2006). Comorbidity of anxiety with eating disorders and OCD. Am. J. Psychiatry 163 327–329. [PubMed] []
  • Södersten P., Bergh C., Leon M., Brodin U., Zandian M. (2017). Cognitive behavior therapy for eating disorders versus normalization of eating behavior. Physiol. Behav. 174 178–190. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Södersten P., Bergh C., Leon M., Zandian M. (2016). Dopamine and anorexia nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 60 26–30. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Södersten P., Bergh C., Zandian M. (2006). Psychoneuroendocrinology of anorexia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31 1149–1153. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.09.006 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Södersten P., Bergh C., Zandian M., Ioakimidis I. (2011). Obesity and the brain. Med. Hypotheses 77 371–373. [PubMed] []
  • Södersten P., Bergh C., Zandian M., Ioakimidis I. (2014). Homeostasis in anorexia nervosa. Front. Neurosci. 8:234. 10.3389/fnins.2014.00234 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Södersten P., Brodin U., Sjöberg J., Zandian M., Bergh C. (2019a). Treatment outcomes for eating disorders in Sweden: data from the national quality registry. BMJ Open 9:e024179. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024179 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Södersten P., Brodin U., Zandian M., Bergh C. (2019b). Eating behavior and the evolutionary perspective on anorexia nervosa. Front. Neurosci. 13:596 10.3389/fnins.2019.00596 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Södersten P., Nergårdh R., Bergh C., Zandian M., Scheurink A. (2008). Behavioral neuroendocrinology and treatment of anorexia nervosa. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 29:445–462. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.06.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Stephens D. W., Krebs J. (1986). Foraging theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. []
  • Stoljar D. (2017). Physicalism. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2017/entries/physicalism (accessed September 3, 2019). []
  • Striedter G. F., Avise J. C., Ayala F. J. (2012). In the light of evolution VI: brain and behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109(Suppl. 1), 10607–10611. [PMC free article] [PubMed] []
  • Stulz N., Hepp U., Gächter C., Martin-Soelch C., Spindler A., Milos G. (2013). The severity of ADHD and eating disorder symptoms: a correlational study. BMC Psychiatry 13:44. 10.1186/1471-244X-13-44 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Sultson H., van Meer F., Sanders N., van Elburg A. A., Danner U. N., Hoek H. W., et al. (2016). Associations between neural correlates of visual stimulus processing and set-shifting in ill and recovered women with anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 255 35–42. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.07.004 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Svanborg P., Åsberg M. (1994). A new self-rating scale for depression and anxiety states based on the comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 89 21–28. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1994.tb01480.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Svedlund N. E., Norring C., Ginsberg Y., von Hausswolff-Juhlin Y. (2017). Symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) among adult eating disorder patients. BMC Psychiatry 17:19. 10.1186/s12888-016-1093-1 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Svensson P., Romaniello A., Wang K., Arendt-Nielsen L., Sessle B. J. (2006). One hour of tongue-task training is associated with plasticity in corticomotor control of the human tongue musculature. Exp. Brain Res. 173 165–173. 10.1007/s00221-006-0380-3 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Swinbourne J. M., Touyz S. W. (2007). The co-morbidity of eating disorders and anxiety disorders: a review. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. J. Eat. Disord. Assoc. 15 253–274. 10.1002/erv.784 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Tenconi E., Degortes D., Clementi M., Collantoni E., Pinato C., Forzan M., et al. (2016). Clinical and genetic correlates of decision making in anorexia nervosa. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 38 327–337. 10.1080/13803395.2015.1112878 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Tryon W. W. (2018). Mediators and mechanisms. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 6 619–628. []
  • Tucker T. (2007). The Great Starvation Experiment: Ancel Keys and the Men Who Starved for Science. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. []
  • Ungar P. S. (2017). Evolution’s Bite: A Story of Teeth, Diet, and Human Origins. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. []
  • University of Minnesota (2018). The Minnesota Semistarvation Experiment. Available at: http://www.epi.umn.edu/cvdepi/video/the-minnesota-semistarvation-experiment/ (accessed September 3, 2019). []
  • Varga S. (2012). Evolutionary psychiatry and depression: testing two hypotheses. Med. Health Care Philos. 15 41–52. 10.1007/s11019-010-9305-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Watson H. J., Yilmaz Z., Thornton L. M., Hübel C., Coleman J. R. I., Gaspar H. A., et al. (2019). Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nat. Genet. 51 1207–1214. 10.1038/s41588-019-0439-2 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Weele C. M. V., Siciliano C. A., Matthews G. A., Namburi P., Izadmehr E. M., Espinel I. C., et al. (2018). Dopamine enhances signal-to-noise ratio in cortical-brainstem encoding of aversive stimuli. Nature 563 397–401. 10.1038/s41586-018-0682-1 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Wegner D. M. (2003). The mind’s best trick: how we experience conscious will. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7 65–69. 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00002-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Wegner D. M. (2004). Précis of the illusion of conscious will. Behav. Brain Sci 27 649–659. [PubMed] []
  • Wegner D. M. (2018). The Illusion of Conscious Will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. []
  • Wegner D. M., Wheatley T. (1999). Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. Am. Psychol. 54 480–492. 10.1037//0003-066x.54.7.480 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Weinbach N., Sher H., Lock J. D., Henik A. (2017). Attention networks in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 27 343–351. 10.1007/s00787-017-1057-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Whitehouse H., François P., Savage P. E., Currie T. E., Feeney K. C., Cioni E., et al. (2019). Complex societies precede moralizing gods throughout world history. Nature 568 226–229. 10.1038/s41586-019-1043-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Willcutt E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurother. J. Am. Soc. Exp. Neurother. 9 490–499. 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Willcutt E. G., Nigg J. T., Pennington B. F., Solanto M. V., Rohde L. A., Tannock R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121 991–1010. 10.1037/a0027347 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Willsey A. J., Morris M. T., Wang S., Willsey H. R., Sun N., Teerikorpi N., et al. (2018). The psychiatric cell map initiative: a convergent systems biological approach to illuminating key molecular pathways in europsychiatric disorders. Cell 174 505–520. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.016 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Wray N. R., Wijmenga C., Sullivan P. F., Yang J., Visscher P. M. (2018). Common disease is more complex than implied by the core gene omnigenic model. Cell 173 1573–1580. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.05.051 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Wu K. D. (2008). Eating disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a dimensional approach to purported relations. J. Anxiety Disord. 22 1412–1420. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.02.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Yilmaz Z., Halvorsen M., Bryois J., Yu D., Thornton L. M., Zerwas S., et al. (2018). Examination of the shared genetic basis of anorexia nervosa and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 10.1038/s41380-018-0115-4 [Epub ahead of print]. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Yilmaz Z., Javaras K. N., Baker J. H., Thornton L. M., Lichtenstein P., Bulik C. M., et al. (2017). Association between childhood to adolescent attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom trajectories and late adolescent disordered eating. J. Adolesc. Health Off. Publ. Soc. Adolesc. Med. 61 140–146. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.04.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Yousif S. R., Aboody R., Keil F. C. (2019). The Illusion of consensus: a failure to distinguish between true and false consensus. Psychol. Sci. 30 1195–1204. 10.1177/0956797619856844 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zandian M. (2009). You Are How You Eat. Ph.D thesis Karolinska Institutet, Solna. []
  • Zandian M., Holmstedt E., Larsson A., Bergh C., Brodin U., Södersten P. (2017). Anxiolytic effect of warmth in anorexia nervosa. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 135 266–267. 10.1111/acps.12691 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zandian M., Ioakimidis I., Bergh C., Södersten P. (2007). Cause and treatment of anorexia nervosa. Physiol. Behav. 92 283–290. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.052 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zandian M., Ioakimidis I., Bergh C., Södersten P. (2009). Linear eaters turned decelerated: reduction of a risk for disordered eating? Physiol. Behav. 96 518–521. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.11.017 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zastrow A., Kaiser S., Stippich C., Walther S., Herzog W., Tchanturia K., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 166 608–616. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050775 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zeeck A., Herpertz-Dahlmann B., Friederich H.-C., Brockmeyer T., Resmark G., Hagenah U., et al. (2018). Psychotherapeutic treatment for anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 9:158. 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00158 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zijlmans J., Marhe R., van der Ende J., Verhulst F. C., Popma A., Tiemeier H., et al. (2017). Children with obsessive-compulsive symptomology in the general population: different subtypes? J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. JDBP 38 476–482. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000467 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []
  • Zipfel S., Giel K. E., Bulik C. M., Hay P., Schmidt U. (2015). Anorexia nervosa: aetiology, assessment, and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry 2 1099–1111. 10.1016/s2215-0366(15)00356-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef] []

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

Comments are closed.