Does a narcissism epidemic exist in modern western societies? Comparing narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany

Does a narcissism epidemic exist in modern western societies? Comparing narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany

OPEN ACCESS

Citation: Vater A, Moritz S, Roepke S (2018) Does a narcissism epidemic exist in modern western societies? Comparing narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany. PLoS ONE 13(1): e0188287. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0188287

Editor: Tzen-Yuh Chiang, National Cheng Kung University, TAIWAN

Received: November 14, 2016

Accepted: November 4, 2017

Published: January 24, 2018

 

Copyright: © 2018 Vater et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability Statement: Data cannot be made publicly available to protect patient confidentiality. Interested, qualified researchers can request the data by contacting the Ethics Committee of Charite´- Universita¨tsmedizin Berlin at ethikkommission@charite.de.

Funding: The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

 

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Does a narcissism epidemic exist in modern western societies? Comparing narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany

Aline Vater1,2,3*, Steffen Moritz4, Stefan Roepke3

1  Freie Universita¨ t Berlin, Department of Education and Psychology, Berlin, Germany, 2  University of Potsdam, Department of Psychology, Potsdam, Germany, 3  Charite´–Universita¨ tsmedizin Berlin, Campus Benjamin Franklin, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Berlin, Germany, 4 University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Department for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Hamburg, Germany

 

* aline.vater@fu-berlin.de

 

 

Abstract

Narcissism scores are higher in individualistic cultures compared with more collectivistic cultures. However, the impact of sociocultural factors on narcissism and self-esteem has not been well described. Germany was formerly divided into two different social systems, each with distinct economic, political and national cultures, and was reunified in 1989/90. Between 1949 and 1989/90, West Germany had an individualistic culture, whereas East Germany had a more collectivistic culture. The German reunification provides an exceptional opportunity to investigate the impact of sociocultural and generational differences on narcissism and self-esteem. In this study, we used an anonymous online survey to assess grandiose narcissism with the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) to assess grandiose and vulnerable aspects of narcissism, and self-esteem with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE) in 1,025 German individuals.

Data were analyzed according to age and place of birth. Our results showed that grandiose narcissism was higher and self-esteem was lower in individuals who grew up in former West Germany compared with former East Germany. Further analyses indicated no significant differences in grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcissism or self-esteem in individuals that entered school after the German reunification (::: 5 years of age in 1989). In the middle age cohort (6–18 years of age in 1989), significant differences in vulnerable narcissism, grandi- ose narcissism and self-esteem were observed. In the oldest age cohort (> 19 years of age in 1989), significant differences were only found in one of the two scales assessing grandi- ose narcissism (NPI). Our data provides empirical evidence that sociocultural factors are associated with differences in narcissism and self-esteem.

 

Introduction

Are modern capitalistic cultures nurturing narcissism? Sociocultural changes are frequently proposed to be central mechanisms contributing to increasing narcissism [1]. The majority of empirical studies on the associations between sociocultural changes and narcissism relied on student samples from the US. In the present study, we examined an exceptional case in human history: The division and reunification of Germany. We investigated whether individuals exposed to these two different social systems (with distinct economic, political and national cultures) differed in grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, and self-esteem.

 

Empirical evidence for the narcissism epidemic

Narcissism is increasing in modern Western societies and this has been referred to as a “narcis- sism epidemic” [1]. The endorsement rate for the statement “I am an important person” has increased from 12% in 1963 to 77–80% in 1992 in adolescents [2]. Recently published books feature more self-centered language compared with earlier publications. For instance, the per- sonal pronouns I and me are used more frequently than we and us [3]. Moreover, the use of narcissistic phrases such as “I am the greatest” has increased between 1960 and 2008 [4]. The rise of narcissism is also reflected in more self-focused song lyrics [5] and a stronger orienta- tion towards fame in TV shows [6]. These observations suggest that narcissistic expressions within individualistic cultures have become more frequent.

Scores of self-reported grandiose narcissism, assessed by the Narcissistic Personality Inven- tory (NPI), have increased [7, 8]. Twenge and Campbell reported a significant increase in NPI scores in a cross-temporal meta-analysis of American college students between 1979 and 2006 [9]. NPI scores were 30% higher in the most recent cohort compared with the first cohort. Fur- ther analyses between 2002 and 2007 excluded any confounding effects of ethnicity. Taken together with other studies, these findings confirmed that NPI scores are increasing over time in American college students ([10], but also see [13]).

Evidence for an increase in narcissism in Western societies has predominantly been pro- vided by the same research group. In these studies, narcissism was consistently measured using the NPI, which has received much criticism [1012]. Shortcomings of the NPI include a restriction to grandiose aspects of narcissism [13] and problems with validity [14]. Moreover, the NPI is constructed based on the clinical definition of Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD) in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) produced by the American Psychiatric Association [15]. Nevertheless, Raskin and Terry developed the NPI to assess individual differences in nonclinical populations. The authors have themselves admitted that the NPI fails to capture important psychological and behavioral dimensions that are inherent to pathological narcissism [7]. This means that high NPI scores may represent a non-distressed, self-confident version of grandiose narcissism [13]. However, recent research shows that the NPI provided a strong match to expert ratings of DSM-IV-TR NPD and grandiose narcissism, compared to other narcissism inventories [16, 17].

The Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) has been developed to capture both grandiose and vulnerable aspects of narcissism [13]. PNI scores are thought to be closely related to the clinical diagnosis of NPD, although this has not yet been tested in narcissistic patients. How- ever, recent evidence indicates that the PNI is a useful tool for assessing more pathological aspects of narcissism [18].

Self-esteem, defined as global evaluation of the self [19], is related to narcissism [14, 2022].

However, recent data provide evidence that narcissism differs from self-esteem in various domains, such as its phenotype, its consequences, its development, and its origins [23]. Self- esteem and narcissism have distinct impacts on outcome measures [21, 2428]. Narcissism and high self-esteem both include positive self-evaluations, but the entitlement, exploitation, sense of superiority, and negative evaluation of others that are associated with narcissism are not necessarily observed in individuals with high self-esteem [29]. Self-esteem is increasing in the Western world. Middle school students from the United States had markedly higher self- esteem scores in the mid-2000s compared with the late 1980s [30]. Moreover, the self-esteem of college students increased substantially between 1968 and 1994 [31].

 

Sociocultural environments and narcissism

The development of personality traits is closely related to the cultural environment [3235]. Cultural environments can be classified as individualistic or collectivistic [36, 37]. Individualis- tic cultures encourage a stronger focus on the self, whereas collectivistic cultures emphasize the importance of social values. Narcissism contains a strong focus on the self, accompanied by a high need for admiration, and grandiose fantasies [38, 39]; therefore members of individ- ualistic cultures may be more narcissistic than individuals from collectivistic cultures [40].

Some studies have provided empirical evidence that individualistic values can lead to nar- cissism [41]. Individuals from the United States, which has a more individualistic culture, have higher grandiose narcissism scores (NPI) compared with individuals from Asian countries and the Middle East, which have a more collectivistic culture [40]. Likewise, Chinese people have lower self-enhancement scores than individuals from more individualistic Western socie- ties, such as the United States, United Kingdom, Canada, Australia, and Europe [42].

Lower self-esteem has been reported in individuals from East Asia than Western cultures [43]. Similarly, Japanese college students had lower self-esteem scores than Westerners [4447]. Lower self-esteem in collectivistic societies may be due to the fact that importance is not focused on the self but rather given to the group. Taken together, these findings suggest that individual differences in self-esteem and narcissism may be explained by distinguishable char- acteristics inherent to different cultures.

 

The effect of age on narcissism

Past research indicates that narcissism decreases with age. Younger individuals have higher narcissism scores [4850] than older individuals. Wilson and Sibley identified a curvilinear effect of age on NPI scores in the general New Zealand population [51]; 9.4% of individuals in their twenties reported symptoms of NPD, compared with 3.2% of individuals older than 65 years. Hypersensitive narcissism is a variant of pathological narcissism and can be measured by the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale [52]. Scores using this scale correlated negatively with age [53].

Empirical research provides evidence that entering adulthood during a recession tempers later narcissism, i.e., participants who come of age during tumultuous economic times are less likely to endorse narcissism relative to people who enter adulthood in more prosperous times [54, 55]. Bianchi argues that economic conditions shape later attitudes and values that reflect the prevailing concerns of the time. Narcissistic self-focus may be viable only when people are not reliant on others to satisfy their basic needs during economic prosperous times [56, 57]. In line with this, in Finnish 18 year old man born between 1962 and 1976 a steady increase in per- sonality traits that are associated with higher earnings in later life (e.g., self-confidence, socia- bility, leadership motivation) has been observed [58].

Self-esteem also changes with age. Latent growth curve analyses have revealed that self- esteem increases with age, peaks at 60 years, and then declines into old age (2: 70 years) [5961]. According to Orth and colleagues, individuals establish their sense of self, including their roles, values, and interests during adolescence and young adulthood [59]. Self-esteem increases with age as personality traits mature and social roles begin to manifest. It then decreases dur- ing old age because individual traits (e.g., physical strength) and social surroundings (e.g., empty nest syndrome, loss of partner) begin to change. Another study shows that birth cohort has a measurable influence on self-esteem through its interaction with age. Participants born in later years (e.g., 1960) score higher in self-esteem than participants born in earlier years (e.g., 1920) [62]. Moreover, the authors compared participants of the same age in 1986 versus 2002 from the Americans’ Changing Lives (ACL) cohort-sequential study. According to their results, participants of the same age in 2002 scored higher in self-esteem than those in 1986. The authors argue that cultural change in the form of cohort and time period have to be con- sidered when assessing self-esteem in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies.

 

Rationale of the present study

Cultural transformation towards more individualistic values in Western societies has been blamed for the rise in narcissism [34, 63]. However, evidence for an increase in narcissism has largely come from NPI scores of college student cohorts collected between the late 1970s and 2010 [63]. Many researchers have argued that this is insufficient evidence for a narcissism epi- demic in Western societies [64, 65]. Moreover, the NPI has been criticized as a measure of nar- cissism [13, 14]. To address these concerns, we have included evaluation by the PNI in our study, which assesses grandiose and vulnerable narcissism and complements information from the NPI [18].

To date, the influence of culture on narcissism has been investigated using cross-temporal birth cohorts [9, 66] or by comparing samples from different cultural environments [40, 42, 67]. These approaches have methodological limitations. Cross-temporal cohorts use the same self-reporting method and can be affected by sampling biases. Trzesniewski and Donnellan reported that population-based inferences cannot be made from cross-temporal meta-analytic studies that are based on convenience sampling [65]. There are also problems with the use of trans-cultural cohorts, including differences in language use and problems with the translation of self-reporting questionnaires used in these studies. Culture is transmitted through language [68]; therefore it is impossible to rule out mediating variables in language use and word mean- ing between cohorts from different cultures.

To address the methodological restrictions of cross-temporal birth cohorts and cross-cul- tural comparisons, we used a historically exceptional research setting: reunified Germany. Ger- many shares the same national culture, but was divided into two independent societies and political systems after World War II. In 1949, the German Democratic Republic (GDR, or East Germany), was founded on the territory of the Russian sector as a communist society with a collectivistic ideology. In the same year, the Federal Republic of Germany (FRG, or West Ger- many) was founded as capitalist society on the territory of the Trizone (including the Ameri- can, British, and French sector). Between 1961 and 1989, interchange between these two German regions was heavily restricted. The Berlin Wall fell in November 1989, marking the beginning of unification. In October 1990, Germany finally became a unified, capitalism-ori- entated society.

The reunification of the socialist GDR and the democratic FRG after more than four decades of separation can be considered a “natural experiment” [6971]. Until 1945, East and West Germany shared central cultural elements, including language, common norms, values, and habits. After 1945, East Germany gradually became a communist society and its people were increasingly exposed to collectivism. In contrast, West Germany retained its capitalistic system and developed a more individualistic culture [72, 73]. As a result, the German popula- tion now consists of a younger generation that grew up under the individualistic system and an older population that were raised either in collectivistic East Germany or individualistic West Germany. Comparing individuals from these two geographical regions may reveal to what degree cultural conditions shape personality across age cohorts.

 

Aims and hypotheses

Main effect for group. The aim of the current study was to compare grandiose narcis- sism, vulnerable narcissism and self-esteem between individuals who grew up in former East and West Germany. First, based on previous findings [40, 42], we hypothesized that grandiose narcissism would be more prevalent in individuals from West Germany, who have been exposed to individualistic views compared with individuals from collectivistic East Germany.

Second, we aimed to determine whether individuals from East and West Germany show differences in vulnerable narcissism. Since narcissistic vulnerability in different cultures has not previously been compared, we did not formulate a direct hypothesis on this matter.

Third, based on previous findings that individuals from collectivistic societies have lower levels of self-esteem [4447], we hypothesized that individuals from East Germany would have lower self-esteem scores (assessed by the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, RSE) compared with individuals from West Germany.

Main effect for age cohort. Learning environments can shape personality [7476]; there- fore we used trichotomized age cohorts to analyze narcissism and self-esteem. The youngest age cohort consisted of individuals ::: 5 years of age in 1989. These individuals had not yet entered school in 1989. The middle age cohort included individuals aged between 6 (entered school in 1989) and 18 (the legal age of consent in 1989) years of age. The oldest age cohort contained individuals that were older than 18 years in 1989.

Individuals from the youngest age cohort were socialized within reunified Germany and shared a similar learning environment; therefore we did not expect to find any differences in grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcissism, and self-esteem between individuals from East and West Germany in this cohort.

Based on the theory that learning environments shape personality [76], we hypothesized that differences in grandiose narcissism and self-esteem would be more pronounced in the middle age cohort, because these individuals were exposed to either individualistic or collectiv- istic conditions during a critical period of personality development. We expected that individ- uals from West Germany would have higher scores for grandiose narcissism and self-esteem compared with individuals from East Germany in this cohort.

We investigated whether grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcissism, and self-esteem are more or less pronounced in individuals from East and West Germany in the oldest cohort. Two studies have shown that grandiose narcissism decreases [48, 50] and self-esteem increases [60, 61] in later life, suggesting that group differences may be less pronounced in the oldest cohort. However, narcissism and self-esteem have not been compered across age cohorts; therefore we did not formulate a direct hypothesis for the main effects.

 

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

A total of 1,025 individuals (age in years: Mage = 38.3, SD = 12.48, range: 18–83; males 31.2%, females 68.8%) from Germany were recruited for this study by advertisements on a social net- working site. Participants were invited to complete an online study and were given the oppor- tunity to obtain an e-book as an incentive. All procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the Charite´ Berlin (EA04/065/06). All participants provided written informed consent by checking a box that stated they agreed to participate in the study; participants could only then proceed with the study. Afterwards, all participants had to insert their age, their level of education (i.e., no school degrees/German: keinen Schulabschluss; secondary modern school qualification 9th class degree/German: Hauptschulabschluss; secondary mod- ern school qualification 10th class degree/German: Realschulabschluss; academic high school degree/German: Abitur; University degree/German: Hochschulabschluss) and town in which they grew up. A research assistant used an online map service to locate whether towns were part of former GDR or FRG.

Three hundred and forty-three participants grew up in the territory of the former GDR and 682 individuals grew up in the territory of the FRG before 1990. Regarding education, two individuals did not have a school degree, 82 had a secondary school degree, 177 had a gradua- tion diploma, 595 had a university degree, and 164 received vocational training. In terms of employment, 72 participants were unemployed, 691 were employed, 223 were students, and 34 were retired. Five individuals did not provide information on their education and occupation.

Trichotomized groups were created based on the age of participants when the Berlin Wall fell in 1989. The youngest cohort (::: 5 years of age in 1989) included 29.7% of the total partici- pants, the middle age cohort (aged 6–18 years in 1989) contained 35.5% of the total partici- pants, and the older cohort (aged 19–41 years in 1989) comprised 34.8% of the total participants.

 

Measures

Grandiose narcissism (NPI). We used a 15-item version of the NPI [77], which is the most widely used self-report measure of narcissism [7, 8, 78, 79]. The NPI primarily measures grandiose aspects of narcissism [13, 80], based on the criteria for pathological narcissism speci- fied in the DSM III [15]. The coefficient alpha was 0.76.

Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (PNI). We used the German version of the PNI [13, 18], which contains 54 items for measuring grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. The PNI is comprised of seven subscales: exploitativeness (EXP, seven items), grandiose fantasy (GF, seven items), self-sacrificing self-enhancement (SSSE, six items), entitlement rage (ER, eight items), devaluing (DEV, seven items), contingent self-esteem (CSE, 12 items), and hiding the self (HS, seven items). Each item is scored on a six-point scale ranging from zero (not at all like me) to five (very much like me). The coefficient alpha was 0.96 for the total score, 0.71 for the grandiose factor (including the EXP, GF, ER and SSSE subscales [13] and 0.84 for the vulnera- ble factor (including the CSE, HS, and DEV subscales [13]. The alphas for the subscales ranged between 0.83 (SSSE) and 0.93 (CSE).

Self-esteem. The RSE [19, 79] is a 10-item self-report measure of global self-esteem. Items are rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The coeffi- cient alpha in this sample was 0.90.

 

Results

Descriptive statistics

S1 Table presents the descriptive statistics and group differences between participants from East and West Germany across age classes. S2 Table shows the intercorrelations of all mea- sures. There were no significant differences in gender and years of education between the East and West Germany groups. In both groups, the NPI scores correlated positively with PNI grandiosity scores, but negatively with PNI vulnerability scores. Self-esteem correlated posi- tively with NPI narcissism and negatively with grandiosity and vulnerability assessed by the PNI. Using the Fisher r-to-z transformation, we calculated whether the correlations between NPI, PNI and RSE significantly differ between East and West Germany in each age class.

There were no significant differences in correlations for all measures in each age class.

 

Group differences

Participants who grew up in West Germany had lower self-esteem scores (RSE) and higher grandiose narcissism scores (NPI and PNI-G) compared with participants who grew up in East Germany (see S1 Table). Although the sum score of all facets of vulnerable narcissism did not differ between groups across age classes, participants from West Germany had by trend higher scores in the HS facet of vulnerable narcissism compared with individuals from East Germany. There were no significant differences in other PNI-V subscales between the two groups (i.e., CSE, and DEV).

In summary, individuals from West Germany scored higher on narcissistic grandiosity compared with participants from East Germany, but no differences in narcissistic vulnerability were observed. Participants who grew up in West Germany were significantly older than par- ticipants who grew up in East Germany. However, controlling for age and gender did not alter the results for differences in self-esteem, grandiose narcissism, and vulnerable narcissism.

Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances was statistically nonsignificant, which indicates that the group variances are equal in the population.

 

Age cohort effect

To determine whether narcissism and self-esteem is different between individuals who grew up before or after the German unification, we performed a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) according to age (young vs. middle vs. old) and group (East Germany vs. West Germany) (see S3 Table). These analyses revealed that grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcis- sism, and self-esteem were different between age cohorts. Further, a group-effect was predomi- nant in the middle age group; individuals who grew up in West Germany had higher grandiose narcissism and vulnerable narcissism scores and lower self-esteem scores compared with individuals in the same age group who grew up in East Germany. Individuals in the mid- dle age group from West Germany had higher grandiose narcissism on three different PNI-G subscales (EXP, GF, and ER facets) as well as one PNI vulnerability subscale (HS facet) com- pared with participants from East Germany in the same age group (see S3 Table).

There were no significant effects in the youngest age cohort. Individuals in the oldest age group from West Germany had higher NPI scores compared with participants from East Ger- many in the same age group. The principal findings related to grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcissism, and self-esteem in all three age classes are illustrated in S1 Fig. Controlling for age and gender does not alter the pattern of results.

 

Discussion

This study takes advantage of the natural experiment [69, 70] created by the division and reunification of Germany to examine whether political systems affect narcissism and self- esteem. We compared measures of grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcissism, and self- esteem between individuals who grew up in the territory of former West and East Germany. We hypothesized that individuals who grew up in the individualistic and capitalistic society of West Germany would have higher grandiose narcissism, and self-esteem scores than individu- als who were raised in the collectivistic and socialist society of East Germany. By analyzing a large, heterogeneous community sample, we demonstrated that individuals from former West Germany have higher grandiose narcissism scores than individuals who grew up in former East Germany, in agreement with our hypothesis. In contrast to our hypothesis, individuals from former East Germany had higher self-esteem than individuals from former West Ger- many. These findings were predominantly detected in the middle age cohort and were largely absent in the youngest and oldest cohorts. In the following, we discuss our findings in relation to the proposed narcissism epidemic [1].

 

Why were differences in narcissism predominant in the middle age cohort?

Youngest age cohort. Personality traits emerge during childhood and adolescence and become more stable in adulthood [81, 82]. Political systems may affect the educational system, therefore influence personality development [83]. Individuals from the youngest cohort (::: 5 years old in 1989) grew up under a common political and cultural system in the capitalism-ori- entated society of reunified Germany. In line with this, we found no significant differences in any study measures between individuals who grew up in East or West Germany.

Middle age cohort. Participants in the middle age cohort (6–18 years old in 1989) grew up either in collectivistic East Germany or individualistic West Germany. Grandiose narcis- sism and vulnerable narcissism scores were higher in individuals from West Germany com- pared to East Germany in this age group.

Interestingly, individuals raised in West Germany scored higher on three grandiose sub- scales of the PNI: EXP (interpersonal manipulativeness), GF (compensatory fantasies of suc- cess and recognition), and ER (anger in response to unmet expectations that they feel entitled to).

Moreover, significant differences were observed in the HS subscale (vulnerable subscale of the PNI: concealing faults and needs from others), but not in the other two vulnerable sub- scales (CSE, DEV). These observations are in line with previous studies arguing that grandiose (but not necessarily vulnerable) narcissism has increased in Western societies [63]. Further- more, our study sample may represent non-clinical individuals, which may explain why no sig- nificant differences were observed in two of the three vulnerable PNI subscales in the middle age cohort. There is previously published data showing that vulnerable aspects of the PNI are more strongly associated with psychopathology than grandiose facets [13, 84]. Moreover, gran- diose aspects of the PNI are associated with tendencies to view oneself as active and energetic and one’s life as exciting [84].

Increased grandiose narcissism in middle-aged individuals from West Germany may be explained by social learning theory [85]. Overvaluation of children by their parents has previ- ously been hypothesized as the origin of narcissism in children in individualistic societies [85]. In a prospective study, Brummelman and colleagues confirmed preliminary data [86] that parental overvaluation predicted narcissism in the child later in life [87]. It remains to be con- firmed whether differences in parental overvaluation existed between the former East and West Germany.

Moreover, the work by Bianchi argues that narcissistic self-focus may be viable during eco- nomic prosperous times and tempered by economic recession [54]. Individuals from GDR who came of age during the reunion of Germany faced tumultuous economic times including the loss of job perspectives (e.g., break down of textile industry and agriculture). Moreover, East German wages and pensions continue to be below the West German wage level since uni- fication [88]. Middle-aged individuals from East Germany may score lower on narcissism as they experience lower economic prosperity compared to middle-aged individuals from West Germany.

Finally, data from an internet sample from China assessing sociodemographic factors related to grandiose narcissism (NPI) might help to explain differences in the middle age cohort in our study [48]. The authors found higher socioeconomic status, offspring of one child families compared to multiple children, living in urban area compared to rural area positively related to grandiose narcissism [48]. Up from the mid-seventies East Germany had higher birth-rates compared to West-Germany [89]. Lower socio-economic status in East-Ger- many compared to West-Germany has already been described and continues to exist [88].

Furthermore, urbanization in West-Germany was much faster between 1949 and 1989/90 compared to East-Germany [90].

Oldest age cohort. Grandiose narcissism measured with the PNI-G was higher in partici- pants who grew up in West Germany than East Germany in the oldest age cohort, but no sig- nificant differences were detected in grandiose narcissism measured with the NPI. Prior data has shown that grandiose narcissism measured with the NPI is widely captured by the exploit- ative (EXP) subscale of the PNI grandiosity scales which reflects a manipulative interpersonal orientation [91]. In the current study, the exploitative PNI subscale had highest correlation with the NPI score and was by trend higher in the oldest age cohort who grew up in West Ger- many than East Germany. This discrepancy in grandiose narcissism between the middle and oldest age group may be because narcissism decreases with age [92]. Older individuals have lower narcissism scores [49, 50] and are less susceptible to personality disorders than younger subjects [93]. Individuals from the oldest age group were in their fifties at the time of the study and may have felt an increased sense of empowerment, security, personal growth, and success than younger participants [94]. NPI-assessed grandiose narcissism may be more resistant to change throughout life because of its beneficial aspects. Furthermore, some of the mentioned factors valid for the middle age cohort might be less relevant or not applicable for the oldest age cohort, e.g., differences in birth rate, urbanization, tumultuous economic times during adolescents and early adulthood.

 

Why was self-esteem higher in participants from East Germany?

Our finding that self-esteem was higher in individuals from East Germany contradicts previ- ous reports that individuals from more collectivistic East Asian countries have lower self- esteem than individuals from Western cultures [4347]. We conclude that these earlier find- ings may not apply to East and West Germany. Furthermore, the assumption that individuals from collectivistic East Asian countries have lower self-esteem than individuals from Western cultures has been stressed by theoretical considerations and empirical data. Sedikides and col- leagues argue that one cannot conclude that the level of self-esteem is weaker in East Asian individuals compared to Western individuals [95]. They argue that the desire for self-esteem is pancultural (i.e., cross-cultural invariance), with differential manifestations of self-esteem in different cultures (i.e., cross-cultural variability). They propose that more refined theoretical formulations and self-report instruments with a high cross cultural sensitivity are needed to better understand within-culture variations in self-esteem (including differences between

East-Asian cultures, Latino and Middle-Eastern cultures and other Eastern countries).

Furthermore, teaching children individualistic virtues may contribute to lower self-esteem. According to Tafarodi and Walters, individualistic societies promote achievement-dependent self-esteem [96], i.e., a self-esteem that is threatened by constant social comparisons and the necessity to achieve more than other individuals. In line with this, narcissists perform better when self-enhancement opportunity is high rather than low, while the performance of partici- pants with low narcissism is relatively unaffected by self-enhancement opportunity [97]. Indi- viduals who grew up in West Germany may have lower, achievement-dependent self-esteem (i.e., which may be unrelated to actual achievement). In contrast, collectivistic societies are directed toward maintaining social harmony [42]. Individuals who grew up in East Germany may experience higher self-esteem, because it appears to be more independent from achieve- ments and social comparisons.

Finally, Brummelman and colleagues demonstrated that parental warmth reported by the child was predicative of the child’s self-esteem, whereas parent-reported parental warmth was not [87]. It is possible that child-perceived parental warmth was more prominent in former East Germany compared with former West Germany.

 

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study are the large sample size and the combined assessment of grandiose narcissism, vulnerable narcissism, and self-esteem. Most importantly, this study takes advan- tage of the natural experiment [69, 70] created by the German division and reunification. In this setting, participants share important characteristics (such as language), but are distinct in others (such as sociocultural education). Our findings have advanced our understanding of the sociocultural mechanisms underlying individual differences in narcissism.

However, there are some methodological limitations to our study that could be addressed by future research. First, our study is based on associations, and conclusions about causal effects should be tested using longitudinal designs. Second, our methods for assessing narcis- sism and self-esteem were based on self-reports, which have limited value for assessing person- ality [98]. In future studies, negative behaviors associated with narcissism in real life situations should be compared between individuals raised in the former East and West Germany [99101]. Third, we cannot determine whether our participants answered the online questionnaire honestly. This criticism also applies to offline questionnaires; therefore peer reports or natural- istic observation methods might represent a better approach towards investigating the inter- personal behavior of narcissists [102, 103]. Future studies may use paper pencil tests and more conventional sampling methods. Fourth, we cannot exclude confounding differences between the East German and West German cohorts. Individuals who grew up in the former East Ger- many experienced a transition from a collectivistic communist society to an individualistic society, which may have affected their narcissism and self-esteem. Moderating variables, such as differences in parenting styles should be investigated in future studies [87].

 

Conclusions

In summary, our study provides preliminary evidence that sociocultural factors contribute to differences in narcissism. Although we cannot pinpoint any causal relationships, we believe that our study sheds further light on sociological trends and justifies further investigation into the alleged narcissism epidemic.

 

Supporting information

S1 Table. Descriptive statistics and MANCOVA effect for group differences between East and West Germany across age classes (controlled for age). NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory; PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory; PNI-G/-V = Pathological Narcissism Inventory grandiose/vulnerable narcissism (according to Pincus et al. 2009);

EXP = Exploitativeness; SSSE = Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement; HS = Hiding the Self; GF = Grandiose Fantasy; DEV = Devaluing; ER = Entitlement Rage; RSE = Rosenberg Self- Esteem Scale; only RSE, PNI and NPI Scales are controlled for age; d = effect size Cohens d. (DOCX)

S2 Table. Correlations of all variables. N = 1,025; intercorrelations for individuals from for- mer East Germany above diagonal (N = 343), intercorrelations for individuals from former West Germany below diagonal (N = 682); NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory;

PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory; PNI-G/-V = Pathological Narcissism Inventory grandiose/vulnerable narcissism; EXP = Exploitativeness; SSSE = Self-Sacrificing Self- Enhancement; HS = Hiding the Self; GF = Grandiose Fantasy; DEV = Devaluing;

ER = Entitlement Rage; RSE = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Descriptive statistics and MANOVA effect for group differences between East and West Germany across age classes. NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory;

PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory; PNI-G/-V = Pathological Narcissism Inventory grandiose/vulnerable narcissism; EXP = Exploitativeness; SSSE = Self-Sacrificing Self- Enhancement; HS = Hiding the Self; GF = Grandiose Fantasy; DEV = Devaluing;

ER = Entitlement Rage; RSE = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; d = effect size Cohens d. (DOCX)

S1 Fig. (1a) Age cohort effect for self-esteem (RSE). (1b) Age cohort effect for narcissism (NPI). (1c) Age cohort effect for grandiose narcissism (PNI-G). (1d) Age cohort effect for vulnerable narcissism (PNI-V). NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory; PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory; PNI-G/-V = Pathological Narcissism Inventory grandiose/vulnerable narcissism; * p < .05, ** p < .01.

(TIF)

S1 File. Ethics statement.

(PDF)

 

 

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Dr. Ramzi Fatfouta and three unknown reviewers for their valuable comments.

 

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Aline Vater, Stefan Roepke.

Data curation: Aline Vater, Steffen Moritz, Stefan Roepke.

Formal analysis: Aline Vater, Stefan Roepke.

Investigation: Aline Vater, Stefan Roepke.

Methodology: Aline Vater, Stefan Roepke.

Project administration: Aline Vater, Stefan Roepke.

Resources: Aline Vater.

Software: Aline Vater.

Writing – original draft: Aline Vater.

Writing – review & editing: Aline Vater, Steffen Moritz, Stefan Roepke.

 

 

References

  1. Twenge JM, Campbell The narcissism epidemic: Living in the age of entitlement. New York, NY, US: Free Press; 2009.
  2. Newsom CR, Archer RP, Trumbetta S, Gottesman Changes in adolescent response patterns on the MMPI/MMPI-A across four decades. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003; 81(1):74–84. doi: 10. 1207/S15327752JPA8101_07. 2003-99679-007. PMID: 12842804
  1. Twenge JM, Campbell WK, Gentile Changes in pronoun use in American books and the rise of indi- vidualism, 1960–2008. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2013; 44(3):406–15. doi: 10.1177/ 0022022112455100. 2013-06649-005.
  2. Twenge JM, Campbell WK, Gentile B. Increases in individualistic words and phrases in American books, 1960–2008. PLoS 2012; 7(7):e40181. Epub 2012/07/19. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0040181 PMID: 22808113; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3393731.
  3. DeWall CN, Pond RS , WK, JM. Tuning in to psychological change: Linguistic markers of psycholog- ical traits and emotions over time in popular U.S. song lyrics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 2011; 5(3):200–7. doi: 10.1037/a0023195. 2011-05681-001.
  4. Uhls Y, Greenfield The rise of fame: An historical content analysis. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace. 2011; 5:article 1.
  5. Raskin R, Terry A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and fur- ther evidence of its construct validity. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1988; 54(5):890– 902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890 PMID: 3379585
  6. Raskin RN, Hall A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological Reports. 1979; 45(2):590-. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590. 1981-08131-001. PMID: 538183
  7. Twenge JM, Campbell Increases in positive self-views among high school students: Birth-cohort changes in anticipated performance, self-satisfaction, self-liking, and self-competence. Psychological Science. 2008; 19(11):1082–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02204.x. 2008-18156-005. PMID: 19076477
  8. Cain NM, Pincus AL, Ansell Narcissism at the crossroads: Phenotypic description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality psychology, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008; 28(4):638–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.006. 2008-03365-006. PMID: 18029072
  9. Rosenthal SA, Montoya RM, Ridings LE, Rieck SM, Hooley Further evidence of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory’s validity problems: A meta-analytic investigation—response to Miller, Maples, and Campbell (this issue). Journal of Research in Personality. 2011; 45(5):408–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp. 2011.06.004. 2011-20188-003.
  10. Rosenthal SA, Hooley Narcissism assessment in social–personality research: Does the associa- tion between narcissism and psychological health result from a confound with self-esteem? Journal of Research in Personality. 2010; 44(4):453–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.008. 2010-15068-001.
  11. Pincus AL, Ansell EB, Pimentel CA, Cain NM, Wright AGC, Levy Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Psychological Assessment. 2009; 21(3):365–79. doi: 10. 1037/a0016530. 2009-12887-012. PMID: 19719348
  12. Ackerman RA, Witt EA, Donnellan MB, Trzesniewski KH, Robins RW, Kashy What Does the Nar- cissistic Personality Inventory Really Measure? Assessment. 2011; 18(1):67–87. https://doi.org/10. 1177/1073191110382845 PMID: 20876550
  13. American Psychiatric Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed. Washington DC; 1980.
  14. Miller JD, McCain J, Lynam DR, Few LR, Gentile B, MacKillop J, et A comparison of the criterion validity of popular measures of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder via the use of expert ratings. Psychological Assessment. 2014; 26:958–69. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036613 PMID: 24773036
  15. Miller JD, Hoffman BJ, Gaughan ET, Gentile B, Maples J, Campbell Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: A nomological network analysis. Journal of Personality. 2011; 79:1013–42. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00711.x PMID: 21204843
  16. Morf CC, Schurch E, Kufner A, Siegrist P, Vater A, Back M, et Expanding the Nomological Net of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory: German Validation and Extension in a Clinical Inpatient Sam- ple. Assessment. 2016. Epub 2016/02/14. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115627010 PMID: 26874362.
  17. Rosenberg Society and the adolescent self-image (rev. ed.). Middletown, CT, England: Wesleyan University Press; 1989.
  18. Brown RP, Zeigler-Hill Narcissism and the non-equivalence of self-esteem measures: A matter of dominance? Journal of Research in Personality. 2004; 38(6):585–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2003.11.002. 2004-21109-005.
  19. Paulhus DL, Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH, Tracy Two replicable suppressor situations in person- ality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2004; 39(2):303–28. doi: 10.1207/ s15327906mbr3902_7. 2004-17529-006. PMID: 26804578
  1. Kwan VSY, Kuang LL, Hui Identifying the sources of self-esteem: The mixed medley of benevo- lence, merit, and bias. Self and Identity. 2009; 8(2–3):176–95. doi: 10.1080/15298860802504874. 2009-09772-006.
  2. Brummelman E, Thomaes S, Sedikides Separating narcissism from self-esteem. Current Direc- tions in Psychological Science. 2016; 25:8–13.
  3. Donnellan MB, Trzesniewski KH, Robins RW, Moffitt TE, Caspi Low Self-Esteem Is Related to Aggression, Antisocial Behavior, and Delinquency. Psychological Science. 2005; 16(4):328–35. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01535.x. 2005-03616-012. PMID: 15828981
  4. Tracy JL, Cheng JT, Robins RW, Trzesniewski Authentic and hubristic pride: The affective core of self-esteem and narcissism. Self and Identity. 2009; 8(2–3):196–213. doi: 10.1080/ 15298860802505053. 2009-09772-007.
  5. Orth U, Robins RW, Meier LL, Conger RD. Refining the vulnerability model of low self-esteem and depression: Disentangling the effects of genuine self-esteem and Journal of personality and social psychology. 2016; 110(1):133–49. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000038.supp (Supplemental). 2015- 17758-001. PMID: 25915133
  6. Sedikides C, Rudich EA, Gregg AP, Kumashiro M, Rusbult Are Normal Narcissists Psychologically Healthy?: Self-Esteem Matters. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2004; 87(3):400–16. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.400. 2004-18348-009. PMID: 15382988
  7. Orth U, Luciano EC. Self-esteem, narcissism, and stressful life events: Testing for selection and socialization. Journal of Personality and Social 2015; 109(4):707–21. https://doi.org/10. 1037/pspp0000049 supp (Supplemental). 2015-23486-001. PMID: 26011661
  8. Zeigler-Hill V, Besser A glimpse behind the mask: Facets of narcissism and feelings of self-worth. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2013; 95(3):249–60. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2012.717150. 2013- 14050-002. PMID: 22946774
  9. Gentile B, Twenge JM, Campbell Birth cohort differences in self-esteem, 1988–2008: A cross- temporal meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology. 2010; 14(3):261–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0019919 10.1037/a0019919.supp (Supplemental). 2010-17363-008.
  10. Twenge Time period and birth cohort differences in depressive symptoms in the U.S., 1982–2013. Social Indicators Research. 2015; 121(2):437–54. doi: 10.1007/s11205-014-0647-1. 2014-23687- 001.
  11. Markus H, Kitayama S, Heiman Culture and “basic” psychological principles. In: Higgins E, Kru- glanski A, editors. Social psychology: handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford; 1006. p. 857–913.
  12. Lasch The culture of narcissism. New York: Warner; 1979.
  13. Roberts BW, Helson Changes in culture, changes in personality: The influence of individualism in a longitudinal study of women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997; 72(3):641–51. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.641. 1997-07966-015. PMID: 9120788
  14. Shweder Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press; 1991.
  15. Hofstede Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills CA: Sage Publications; 1980.
  16. Triandis Individualism & collectivism. Boulder, CO, US: Westview Press; 1995.
  17. American Psychiatric Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. text rev. Washington DC; 2000.
  18. American Psychiatric Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington DC; 2013.
  19. Foster JD, Campbell WK, Twenge Individual differences in narcissism: Inflated self-views across the lifespan and around the world. Journal of Research in Personality. 2003; 37(6):469–86. doi: 10. 1016/S0092-6566(03)00026-6. 2003-09807-001.
  20. Paris Modernity and narcissistic personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2014; 5(2):220–6. doi: 10.1037/a0028580. 2012-18549-001. PMID: 22800179
  21. Hepper EG, Sedikides C, Cai H. Self-enhancement and self-protection strategies in China: Cultural expressions of a fundamental human Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2013; 44(1):5–23. doi: 10.1177/0022022111428515. 2012-33261-002.
  22. Heine SJ, Lehman DR, Markus HR, Kitayama Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review. 1999; 106(4):766–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.766. 1999-11924-005. PMID: 10560328
  1. Feather NT, McKee Global self-esteem and attitudes toward the high achiever for Australian and Japanese students. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1993; 56(1):65–76. doi: 10.2307/2786646. 1993- 37493-001.
  2. Kobayashi C, Brown Self-esteem and self-enhancement in Japan and America. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology. 2003; 34(5):567–80. doi: 10.1177/0022022103256479. 2003-07736-008.
  3. Yamaguchi S, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR, Murakami F, Chen D, Shiomura K, et Apparent univer- sality of positive implicit self-esteem. Psychological Science. 2007; 18(6):498–500. doi: 10.1111/j. 1467-9280.2007.01928.x. 2007-09390-006. PMID: 17576261
  4. Schmitt DP, Allik Simultaneous Administration of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in 53 Nations: Exploring the Universal and Culture-Specific Features of Global Self-Esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005; 89(4):623–42. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.623. 2005-13803-014. PMID: 16287423
  5. Cai H, Kwan VSY, Sedikides A sociocultural approach to narcissism: The case of modern China. European Journal of Personality. 2012; 26(5):529–35. doi: 10.1002/per.852. 2012-25718-007.
  6. Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Huang B, Smith SM, et Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV narcissistic personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008; 69(7):1033–45. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0701. 2009-03205-001. PMID: 18557663
  7. Trull TJ, Jahng S, Tomko RL, Wood PK, Sher Revised NESARC personality disorder diagnoses: Gender, prevalence, and comorbidity with substance dependence disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2010; 24(4):412–26. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.4.412. 2010-16406-002. PMID: 20695803
  8. Wilson MS, Sibley ’Narcissism creep?’: Evidence for age-related differences in narcissism in the New Zealand general population. New Zealand Journal of Psychology. 2011; 40(3):89–95. 2012- 16687-008.
  9. Wink Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1991; 61(4):590–7. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.590. 1992-03870-001. PMID: 1960651
  10. Cramer Narcissism through the ages: What happens when narcissists grow older? Journal of Research in Personality. 2011; 45(5):479–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.06.003. 2011-20188-009.
  11. Bianchi Entering adulthood in a recession tempers later narcissism. Psychological Science. 2014; 25:1429–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614532818 PMID: 24815615
  12. Bianchi Assessing the robustness of the relationship between entering adulthood in a recession and narcissism. Psychological Science. 2015; 26:537–8. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614568157 PMID: 25656277
  13. Greenfield Linking social change and developmental change: Shifting pathways of human devel- opment. 45. 2009:401–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014726 PMID: 19271827
  14. Park H, Twenge JM, Greenfield The Great Recession: Implications for adolescent values and behavior. Social Psychological & Personality Science. 2014; 5:310–8.
  15. Jokela M, Pekkarinen T, Sarvima¨ ki M, Tervio¨ M, Uusitalo R. Secular rise in economically valuable per- sonality Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017; 114(25):6527–32. https://doi. org/10.1073/pnas.1609994114 PMID: 28584092
  16. Orth U, Maes J, Schmitt Self-esteem development across the life span: a longitudinal study with a large sample from Germany. Dev Psychol. 2015; 51(2):248–59. Epub 2014/12/09. https://doi.org/10. 1037/a0038481 PMID: 25485608.
  17. Orth U, Trzesniewski KH, Robins Self-esteem development from young adulthood to old age: A cohort-sequential longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010; 98(4):645– 58. doi: 10.1037/a0018769. 2010-05457-009. PMID: 20307135
  18. Orth U, Robins RW, Widaman Life-span development of self-esteem and its effects on important life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2012; 102(6):1271–88. doi: 10.1037/ a0025558. 2011-21756-001. PMID: 21942279
  19. Twenge JM, Carter NT, Campbell Age, time period, and birth cohort differences in self-esteem: Reexamining a cohort-sequential longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Advance online publication http://dxdoiorg/101037/pspp0000122. 2016.
  20. Twenge JM, Foster Birth cohort increases in narcissistic personality traits among American college students, 1982–2009. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2010; 1(1):99–106. doi: 10.1177/ 1948550609355719. 2011-19474-015.
  21. Trull Ruminations on narcissistic personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2014; 5(2):230–1. doi: 10.1037/per0000009. 2014-16207-002. PMID: 24796569
  1. Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan Rethinking ’Generation Me’: A study of cohort effects from 1976– 2006. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010; 5(1):58–75. doi: 10.1177/1745691609356789. 2010-09692-006. PMID: 26162063
  2. Twenge JM, Foster Mapping the scale of the narcissism epidemic: Increases in narcissism 2002– 2007 within ethnic groups. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008; 42(6):1619–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp. 2008.06.014. 2008-17097-025.
  3. Lyons M, Morgan K, Thomas J, Al Hashmi Patterns of parental warmth, attachment, and narcissism in young women in United Arab Emirates and the United Kingdom. Individual Differences Research. 2013; 11(4):149–58. 2014-03581-003.
  4. Triandis Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality. 2001; 69(6):907–24. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.696169. 2001-09460-005. PMID: 11767823
  5. DiNardo Natural experiments and quasi-natural experiments. In: Steven D, Blume L, editors. The new Palgrave Dictionary of Economics 2nd: Palgrave Macmillan; 2008.
  6. Dunning Natural Experiments in the Social Sciences: A Design-Based Approach: Cambridge Uni- versity Press; 2012.
  7. Friehe T, Pannenberg M, Wedow Let Bygones Be Bygones? Socialist Regimes and Personalities in Germany. SOEPpapers. 2015;(776).
  8. Clarke D, Wo¨ lfel U. Remembering the German Democratic Republic. Divided Memory in a United Ger- many: Palgrave Macmillan;
  9. Putnam The evolution of social capital in contemporary societies. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002.
  10. Bandura A, Walters Social learning and personality development: Holt Rinehart and Winston: New York; 1963.
  11. Caspi A, Shiner Personality development. In: D, L, editors. Handbook of child psychology volume 3 Social emotional and personality development 6th ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. p. 300–65.
  12. Trautwein U, Lu¨ dtke O, Ko¨ ller O, Baumert J. Self-esteem, academic self-concept, and achievement: How the learning environment moderates the dynamics of self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social 2006; 90(2):334–49. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.334. 2006-02911-009. PMID: 16536654
  13. Spangenberg L, Romppel M, Bormann B, Hofmeister D, Bra¨ hler E, Strauß B. Psychometrische U¨ ber- pru¨ fung einer Kurzform des Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-15): Dimensionalita¨ t und psycho- metrische Eigenschaften des NPI-15 in einer repra¨ sentativen Bevo¨ lkerungsstichprobe. = A short version of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-15): Dimensionality and psychometric properties in a representative sample of the German PPmP: Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizi- nische Psychologie. 2013; 63(8):341–7. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1333288. PubMed PMID: 2013-27514- 005.
  14. Schu¨ tz A, Marcus B, Sellin I. Die Messung von Narzissmus als Perso¨ nlichkeitskonstrukt: Psychome- trische Eigenschaften einer Lang- und einer Kurzform des Deutschen NPI (Narcissistic Personality Inventory). = Measuring narcissism as a personality construct: Psychometric properties of a long and a short version of the German Narcissistic Personality Diagnostica. 2004; 50(4):202–18. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.50.4.202. 2004-21116-004.
  15. von Collani G, Herzberg Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwetge- fu¨ hl von Rosenberg. = A revised version of the German adaptation of Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale. Zeitschrift fu¨ r Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie. 2003; 24(1):3–7. doi: 10.1024//0170- 1789.24.1.3. 2003-00265-001.
  16. Vater A, Schro¨ der-Abe´ M, Ritter K, Renneberg B, Schulze L, Bosson JK, et al. The Narcissistic Per- sonality Inventory: A Useful Tool for Assessing Pathological Narcissism? Evidence From Patients With Narcissistic Personality Journal of Personality Assessment. 2013; 95(3):301–8. https:// doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.732636 PMID: 23101721
  17. Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC. Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011; 101(4):862–82. doi: 10.1037/a0024950. 2011-18537-001. PMID: 21859226
  18. Specht J, Luhmann M, Geiser On the consistency of personality types across adulthood: Latent profile analyses in two large-scale panel studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2014; 107(3):540–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036863 10.1037/a0036863.supp (Supplemental). 2014- 34128-009. PMID: 25133730
  19. Hess R, Torney-Purta J, Valsiner The Development of Political Attitudes in Children: Aldine Trans- action; 2005.
  1. Thomas KM, Wright AGC, Lukowitsky MR, Donnellan MB, Hopwood Evidence for the criterion validity and clinical utility of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Assessment. 2012; 19(2):135–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191112436664. 2012-14892-001. PMID: 22315481
  2. Millon T, Davis Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1996.
  3. Otway LJ, Vignoles Narcissism and Childhood Recollections: A Quantitative Test of Psychoana- lytic Predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006; 32(1):104–16. doi: 10.1177/ 0146167205279907. 2005-15722-009. PMID: 16317192
  4. Brummelman E, Thomaes S, Nelemans SA, de Castro BO, Overbeek G, Bushman Origins of nar- cissism in children. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015; 112(12):3659–62. 2015-13608-005.
  5. Go¨ rzig B, Gornig M, Werwatz A. Explaining Eastern Germany’s Wage Gap: The Impact of Structural Post-Communist Economies. 2006:449–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14631370500350744.
  6. Bundesamt Geburtentrends und Familiensituation in Deutschland Wiesbaden2012. Available from: https://www.destatis.de/DE/ZahlenFakten/GesellschaftStaat/Bevoelkerung/Geburten/ AktuellGeburtenentwicklung.html.
  7. Ha¨ ussermann H, Neef R. Stadtentwicklung in Ostdeutschland: Soziale und ra¨ umliche Tenden Sozialwissenschaften VVf, editor1996.
  8. Pincus AL, Ansell EB, Pimentel CA, Cain NM, Wright AG, Levy Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Psychological assessment. 2009; 21(3):365–79. Epub 2009/ 09/02. doi: 10.1037/a0016530. 19719348. PMID: 19719348
  9. Edelstein RS, Newton NJ, Stewart Narcissism in midlife: Longitudinal changes in and correlates of women’s narcissistic personality traits. Journal of Personality. 2012; 80(5):1179–204. doi: 10.1111/j. 1467-6494.2011.00755.x. 2012-26355-002. PMID: 22092045
  10. Cohen BJ, Nestadt G, Samuels JF, Romanoski AJ, McHugh PR, Rabins Personality disorder in later life: A community study. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1994; 165(4):493–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp. 165.4.493. 1995-25437-001. PMID: 7804664
  11. Stewart AJ, Ostrove JM, Helson Middle aging in women: Patterns of personality change from the 30s to the 50s. Journal of Adult Development. 2001; 8(1):23–37. doi: 10.1023/A:1026445704288. 2001-17011-003.
  12. Sedikides C, Gaertner L, Cai On the panculturality of self-enhancement and self-protection motiva- tion: The case for the universality of self-esteem. In: Elliot AJ, editor. Advances in Motivation Science
  13. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.; 2015. p. 185–241.
  14. Tafarodi RW, Walters Individualism–collectivism, life events, and self-esteem: A test of two trade- offs. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1999; 29(5–6):797–814. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992 (199908/09)29:5/6<797::AID-EJSP961>3.0.CO;2-S. 1999-03560-015.
  15. Wallace HM, Baumeister The Performance of Narcissists Rises and Falls With Perceived Oppor- tunity for Glory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002; 82(5):819–83. PMID: 12003480
  16. Oltmanns T, Turkheimer Perceptions of self and others regarding pathological personality traits. Personality and psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. p. 71–111.
  17. Back MD, Schmukle SC, Egloff Why are narcissists so charming at first sight? Decoding the narcis- sism–popularity link at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010; 98 (1):132–45. doi: 10.1037/a0016338. 2009-24670-005. PMID: 20053038
  18. Back MD, Baumert A, Denissen JJA, Hartung F-M, Penke L, Schmukle SC, et PERSOC: A unified framework for understanding the dynamic interplay of personality and social relationships. European Journal of Personality. 2011; 25(2):90–107. doi: 10.1002/per.811. 2011-08768-002.
  19. Ku¨ fner ACP, Nestler S, Back MD. The two pathways to being an (un-)popular narcissist. Journal of 2013; 81(2):184–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00795.x. 2013-10044-006. PMID: 22583074
  20. Hoerger Participant dropout as a function of survey length in Internet-mediated university studies: Implications for study design and voluntary participation in psychological research. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2010; 13(6):697–700. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0445. 2010-26270- 013. PMID: 21142995
  21. Joinson A. Social desirability, anonymity, and Internet-based questionnaires. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & 1999; 31(3):433–8. doi: 10.3758/BF03200723. 1999-11751-007.
Comments are closed.