Aggression in borderline personality disorder: A multidimensional model

Aggression in borderline personality disorder: A multidimensional model

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/280219509

 

Article in Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment · July 2015

DOI: 10.1037/per0000098 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS 27 READS 372

 

3 authors, including:

Sabine C Herpertz Universität Heidelberg

94 PUBLICATIONS 4,482 CITATIONS

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Understanding and Breaking the Intergenerational Cycle of Abuse (UBICA) View project

Cortical Thickness and Resting State Cardiac Function Across the Lifespan: A Cross-Sectional Pooled Mega Analysis View project

 

Falk Mancke, Sabine C. Herpertz, and Katja Bertsch

 

University  of Heidelberg

This article proposes a multidimensional model of aggression in borderline personality disorder (BPD) from the perspective of the biobehavioral dimensions of affective dysregulation, impulsivity, threat hypersensitivity, and empathic functioning. It summarizes data from studies that investigated these biobehavioral dimensions using self-reports, behavioral tasks, neuroimaging, neurochemistry as well as psychophysiology, and identifies the following alterations: (a) affective dysregulation associated with prefrontal-limbic imbalance, enhanced heart rate reactivity, skin conductance, and startle response; (b) impulsivity also associated with prefrontal-limbic imbalance, central serotonergic dysfunction, more electroencephalographic slow wave activity, and reduced P300 amplitude in a 2-tone discrimination task;

(c) threat hypersensitivity associated with enhanced perception of anger in ambiguous facial expressions, greater speed and number of reflexive eye movements to angry eyes (shown to be compensated by exogenous oxytocin), enhanced P100 amplitude in response to blends of happy versus angry facial expressions, and prefrontal-limbic imbalance; (d) reduced cognitive empathy associated with reduced activity in the superior temporal sulcus/gyrus and preliminary findings of lower oxytocinergic and higher vasopressinergic activity; and (e) reduced self-other differentiation associated with greater emotional simulation and hyperactivation of the somatosensory cortex. These biobehavioral dimensions can be nicely linked to conceptual terms of the alternative Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5) model of BPD, and thus to a multidimensional rather than a traditional categorical approach.

Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000098.supp

 

 

Introduction

Aggression can be defined as any behavior directed toward another individual that is carried out with the proximate intent to cause harm (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). The high prevalence of aggression in borderline personality disorder (BPD) is demon- strated by data showing that 73% of individuals diagnosed with BPD have engaged in aggressive behaviors over the course of a year (Newhill, Eack, & Mulvey, 2009), 58% have been “occasion- ally or often” involved in physical fights, and 25% have used a weapon against others at some point in their lives (Soloff, Meltzer, & Becker, et al., 2003, p. 154). Additionally, individuals with BPD constitute a major proportion of aggression-prone populations such as prison inmates, with prevalence rates of 30% (Black et al., 2007). Studies have found enhanced aggression in BPD compared with healthy and clinical controls irrespective of whether the inclusion was based on categorical Diagnostic and Statistical

Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition/fourth edition (DSM– III/IV) diagnosis (e.g., Gardner, Leibenluft, O’Leary, & Cowdry, 1991; McCloskey et al., 2009; Soloff, Kelly, Strotmeyer, Malone, & Mann, 2003) or on dimensional severity scores of BPD traits (e.g., Hines, 2008; Ostrov & Houston, 2008; Raine, 1993; Whisman & Schonbrun, 2009). Therefore, aggression has been regarded as a core feature of BPD (e.g., Siever et al., 2002; Skodol et al., 2002).

Aggression is most widely classified into instrumental and re- active forms (e.g., Berkowitz, 1993). Instrumental aggression re- fers to planned, goal-directed behavior, whereas reactive aggres- sion is usually triggered by threats, frustration, or provocation and is strongly associated with negative emotions, particularly anger (e.g., Barratt & Felthous, 2003; Poulin & Boivin, 2000). In BPD, aggression is typically of the reactive type (Blair, 2004; Gardner, Archer, & Jackson, 2012; Herpertz et al., 2001). This has also been confirmed by laboratory tests of aggression, in which BPD patients have been repeatedly found to react more aggressively to provo- cations of a (fictitious) opponent compared with healthy individ- uals (Dougherty, Bjork, Huckabee, Moeller, & Swann, 1999; McCloskey et al., 2009; New et al., 2009). There is broad evidence indicating that aggression in BPD is tightly linked to interpersonal dysfunction, with negative interpersonal events (Herr, Keenan- Miller, Rosenthal, & Feldblum, 2013) and interpersonal problems (Stepp, Smith, Morse, Hallquist, & Pilkonis, 2012) predicting subsequent aggressive behavior in subjects scoring high on BPD traits. Additionally, BPD-associated aggression has been shown to primarily emerge in close relationships, with most aggressive acts directed against significant others or acquaintances (Newhill et al., 2009). BPD traits were also linked to intimate partner violence in young to late middle-aged individuals (Holtzworth-Munroe, Mee- han, Herron, Rehman, & Stuart, 2000; Ross & Babcock, 2009; Weinstein, Gleason, & Oltmanns, 2012).

DSM-5 offers two concepts of BPD: In Section II (diagnostic criteria and codes), DSM-5 provides a broad analogy to the cate- gorical polythetic classification of BPD in DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 663). The alternative model of BPD in Section III (emerging measures and models) uses a hybrid approach, which still conceptualizes BPD categorically, but in terms of multiple personality dimensions: All BPD individuals are located on a spectrum of impairments in personality functioning (differentiated into self and interpersonal functioning) and patho- logical personality traits on the domains of negative affectivity, antagonism, and disinhibition (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 761; see, e.g., Skodol, 2012, for a review).

Our review begins with significant biobehavioral dimensions, meaning fundamental behavioral facets that have been shown to be associated with neurobiological alterations in BPD. The following dimensions have been discussed to underlie aggression in BPD: affective dysregulation, impulsivity, threat hypersensitivity, and empathic functioning. Our approach is to summarize the data related to these biobehavioral dimensions comprising self-reports, behavioral tasks, neuroimaging, neurochemistry, and psychophys- iology.1 We will then provide a working model of aggression in BPD, which illustrates that these biobehavioral dimensions can be nicely linked to conceptual terms of the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD, and thus to a multidimensional rather than a traditional categorical approach. The alternative DSM-5 model could there- fore serve as a suitable starting point for future research on aggression in BPD.

 

Self-Reports and Behavioral Tasks

In this section, we will summarize studies that used self-reports and behavioral tasks to investigate the biobehavioral dimensions in the context of aggression in BPD.

Affective dysregulation plays a key role in theories of reactive aggression, as they regard reactive aggression to be a result of insufficiently controlled negative affect, predominantly anger (Berkowitz, 2003; Davidson et al., 2000; Siever, 2008). Links between affective dysregulation and aggression in BPD can be seen in studies revealing associations between BPD traits and guilt-, resentment-, and irritation-related self-reported forms of aggression that are described as the “more emotional aggressive- ness subdimensions” (Fossati et al., 2004, p. 168). BPD traits also correlated positively with maladaptive forms of emotional coping, such as blaming oneself and venting emotions, which in turn mediated reactive aggression (Gardner, Archer, & Jackson, 2012). Two prospective studies revealed that affective dysregulation fully mediated the relationship between BPD traits and subsequent aggressive behavior in a mixed clinical and community sample (Scott, Stepp, & Pilkonis, 2014) and in a solely clinical sample of BPD patients (Newhill, Eack, & Mulvey, 2012). This emphasizes the high relevance of affective dysregulation for aggression in BPD. Affective dysregulation might also predispose the individual to increased experiences of anger or hostility. This was empirically

demonstrated by Trull et al. (2008), who used experience sampling methodology (i.e., ambulatory and real-time data collection) to show that BPD patients more frequently experienced extreme spikes of hostility than a clinical control group of depressive patients. The experience of anger may ultimately result in aggressive behavior, as recently illustrated in a study showing that the expression of anger, for example, arguing with other people, predicted subsequent aggressive behaviors in individuals scoring high on BPD traits (Stepp et al., 2012). The relevance of anger for aggression in BPD was also supported by the identification of a subgroup of BPD patients in whom anger proneness and aggres- sion were found to co-occur (Hallquist & Pilkonis, 2012).

Reactive aggression has also been considered as a consequence of impulsivity (Coccaro, Sripada, Yanowitch, & Phan, 2011; Gollan, Lee, & Coccaro, 2005; Goodman & New, 2000). Data from BPD patients showed that impulsivity and aggression were positively correlated (e.g., Hollander et al., 2005; Soloff, Meltzer, et al., 2003) and loaded on the same factor in one (Koenigsberg et al., 2001) but not in all studies (Critchfield, Levy, & Clarkin, 2004). Scott et al., (2014) reported that impulsivity did not mediate between BPD traits and aggression. Besides differences in sample characteristics, the latter result might be attributable to the instrument used to assess impulsivity, that is, a composite of different subscales of the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992). As this instrument was designed to assess normative personality traits, it may have failed to capture the aspects of impulsivity specifically related to aggression. Given the strong correlation between impulsivity and anger (García-Forero, Gallardo-Pujol, Maydeu-Olivares, & Andrés-Pueyo, 2009), one might also specu- late that the effect of impulsivity on aggression varies as a function of anger. That is, poor impulse control might only (or particularly) result in aggressive behavior under circumstances of momentarily experienced anger.

Threat hypersensitivity is a central construct in Blair’s model of reactive aggression (Blair, 2004, 2012). Animal studies indicate that aggressive behavior is displayed when a threat is very close and escape is impossible (Blanchard, Blanchard, Takahashi, & Kelley, 1977). In BPD patients, threat hypersensitivity has mainly been studied by means of facial recognition tasks. Results indicate a biased or enhanced perception of social threat cues in BPD patients (Domes, Schulze, & Herpertz, 2009): Compared with healthy controls, they overreported fear when presented with neutral faces (Wagner & Linehan, 1999), perceived ambiguous blends of facial expression as more angry (Domes et al., 2008), and focused more initial attention (von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna et al., 2010a) toward and had difficulties in disengaging from negative facial expressions (von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna et al., 2010b). In addition, in a study using eye tracking, BPD patients were found to have more and faster initial reflexive eye movements toward the eyes of very briefly presented angry faces, thus the most threat- ening and arousing region (Bertsch, Gamer, et al., 2013). These findings suggest that BPD patients misattribute facial emotions, making them susceptible to the experience of threat or provocation, and ultimately to reactive aggression.

 

1 See Table S1 in the supplemental materials for a detailed description of the cited studies, including sample characteristics, methodology, and key findings.

 

Threat hypersensitivity may be one of the sources leading to the interpersonal hypersensitivity of BPD individuals. According to the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD, interpersonal hypersensitivity is the “prone[ness] to feel slighted or insulted” (American Psychiatric As- sociation, 2013, p. 766). It has been linked to aggression in BPD by findings showing that criticism or blame predicted aggressive behav- ior in women scoring high on BPD traits (Herr et al., 2013). Further- more, fearful forms of attachment that are particularly linked to interpersonal hypersensitivity (Gunderson & Lyons-Ruth, 2008) have been associated with reactive aggression in BPD patients (Critchfield, Levy, Clarkin, & Kernberg, 2008). Threat hypersensitivity might also interact with fundamental assumptions of BPD patients, such as seeing the world and others as dangerous and malevolent, which may lead to feelings of threat and could provoke reactive aggression (Arntz, Dreessen, Schouten, & Weertman, 2004; Arntz, 1994; Pretzer, 1990).

Empathic functioning can be differentiated into cognitive empa- thy, affective empathy, and supplemental regulatory mechanisms. Analogously to mentalization or theory of mind, cognitive empathy is regarded as the capacity to infer the mental states of others. Affective empathy captures the emotional engagement with other individuals’ emotional displays. The regulatory mechanisms allow the distinction between emotional reactions of the self and the other, hereafter called self-other differentiation (see, e.g., Decety, 2011; Jeung & Herpertz, 2014, for reviews). Impaired empathic functioning is fundamental to the evolution of human aggression (de Waal, 2012; Decety, 2011; Fonagy, 2003). In empirical terms, cognitive empathy in particular has been associated with reactive aggression (Fossati et al., 2009; Jolliffe & Farrington, 2004; Re- nouf et al., 2010; van Langen et al., 2014). In BPD patients, reduced cognitive empathy was found in studies using self-reports (Harari, Shamay-Tsoory, Ravid, & Levkovitz, 2010; New et al., 2012) or advanced behavioral tasks that approximate real-life social interactions (Dziobek et al., 2011; Preißler, Dziobek, Ritter, Heekeren, & Roepke, 2010; Ritter et al., 2011). The latter tasks— also referred to as ecologically valid tasks— use, for instance, short film clips displaying social interactions, after which participants have to evaluate the intentions, emotions, and thoughts of the interaction partners. Contrary to the finding of reduced cognitive empathy, and despite some inconsistencies (Dziobek et al., 2011), affective empathy seems to be intact in BPD patients (Harari et al., 2010; Mier et al., 2013; New et al., 2012).

First results indicate that BPD patients try excessively to inter- pret other people’s mental states and/or overattribute the intentions of others, which suggests impairments in self-other differentiation (Sharp et al., 2011). In tasks that can be performed via cognitive or affective empathic involvement, such as the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes task“ (Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997), BPD patients outperformed healthy controls (Fertuck et al., 2009; Frick et al., 2012), possibly through (compensatory) over- mobilizing affective empathic strategies, such as emotional simu- lation (Gallese & Goldman, 1998). In line with the theory of greater emotional simulation in BPD, Matzke et al. (2014) found that compared with healthy controls, BPD patients exhibited higher electromyographic activity of the frowning muscle while viewing negative facial expressions of others such as anger, sad- ness, and disgust. Exaggerated emotional simulation, however, impacts negatively on the capability of self-other differentiation, resulting in an affect-dominated and unmediated perception of

others, known as emotional contagion (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009; Schmahl & Herpertz, 2014). This leaves BPD patients “vulnerable to losing a sense of self” (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009, p. 1362) and to being increasingly overwhelmed by others’ mental states. In the context of dysfunctional intimate relationships, which provoke negative emotions of despair, jealousy (Costa & Babcock, 2008), fear of abandonment (Gunderson, 1996), mistrust, and/or shame (Rüsch et al., 2007), this could result in experiences of anger (Peters, Geiger, Smart, & Baer, 2014), threat, frustration, and eventually reactive aggression.

Interestingly, the pattern of deficient empathic functioning of BPD patients is the opposite of that found in individuals with other aggression-prone personality disorders, such as antisocial or psy- chopathic individuals. The latter show intact cognitive empathy but impaired affective empathy (Blair, 2013), favoring instrumental forms of aggression (Blair, Peschardt, Budhani, Mitchell, & Pine, 2006) which have been shown not to be abnormally enhanced in BPD (Herpertz et al., 2001). However, studies directly investigat- ing the relationship between empathic functioning and aggression in BPD are still lacking, and further research is needed to disen- tangle the differential contributions of empathic functioning to aggression in BPD and related disorders.

In the attempt to capture these findings (hereafter related to alterations of the biobehavioral dimensions) within the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD, they can be subsumed under both impair- ments of personality functioning and pathological personality traits (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, pp. 766 –767), and thus the two descriptive levels provided by this classification. Affective dysregulation is closely related to emotional lability, a trait facet of the personality domain negative affectivity. Impulsivity is directly addressed as a trait facet of the personality domain disinhibition. Threat hypersensitivity can be related to both descriptive levels: to the trait facets of emotional lability and hostility on the one hand, and to impairments in interpersonal functioning on the other. Within the latter, it can be attributed to the empathic deficit of BPD patients, namely to interpersonal hypersensitivity and a negatively biased perception of others. The reduced cognitive empathy is encompassed in the description of the empathic deficit of the alternative DSM-5 model. However, reduced self-other differenti- ation, regarded as a facet of poor empathy in neuroscientific models (Decety & Jackson, 2004), is not directly addressed as an empathic deficit in the alternative DSM-5 model. Instead, it is linked to the “markedly impoverished, poorly developed, or un- stable self-image” mentioned as reflecting disturbances of BPD patients’ self-identity.

 

Neuroimaging

Contemporary theories on the neurobiological underpinnings of reactive aggression propose a brain circuitry implicating prefron- tal, limbic, and mesencephalic structures. More specifically, in animals, a pathway has been identified that runs from medial amygdala areas to the medial hypothalamus via the stria terminalis, and from there to the dorsal half of the periaqueductal gray. Neurons within the periaqueductal gray matter are activated, caus- ing the autonomic and motor aspects of reactive aggression. Areas modulating the aggressive response include the hippocampus, the cingulate and prefrontal cortices (Blair, 2004; Gregg & Siegel, 2001). In humans, the understanding of the processes leading to aggression emphasizes the functional relevance of prefrontal and limbic structures. Prefrontal regions, especially the orbital frontal and anterior cingulate cortices, fail to control enhanced affective reactivity of limbic regions such as the amygdala. We will begin this section by describing structural brain abnormalities in BPD patients, and will then summarize results from functional imaging studies.

 

Structural Neuroimaging

Structural neuroimaging studies revealed differences in the brain areas mentioned above in BPD patients relative to healthy controls, with smaller amygdala and hippocampal volumes in BPD patients being the most consistent findings across studies (see Nunes et al., 2009, for a meta-analytic review). Less consistent are findings of smaller gray matter volumes in the anterior cingulate (Hazlett et al., 2005; Minzenberg, Fan, New, Tang, & Siever, 2008; Tebartz van Elst et al., 2003) and in orbital frontal cortices (Brunner et al., 2010; Chanen et al., 2008) of BPD patients when compared with healthy controls. Recently, our group reported a larger gray matter volume in the hypothalamus of female BPD patients compared with healthy women (Kuhlmann, Bertsch, Schmidinger, Thomann, & Herpertz, 2013), a structure which is critically implicated in the regulation of aggressive behavior (Haller, 2013). The hypothalamic volume of BPD patients was positively correlated with scores on the Childhood Trauma Ques- tionnaire (Bernstein & Fink, 1998), suggesting a role of traumatic experiences in volumetric abnormalities of the hypothalamus. In a related vein, Morandotti et al. (2013) demonstrated that gray matter volume loss in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex was specifically associated with aggression in those BPD patients with a history of traumatization, indicating a particular impact of trau- matization on prefrontal regions and in turn aggression. The rele- vance of traumatic experiences for aggression is in line with findings of higher aggressive behavior in patients with posttrau- matic stress disorder (Van Voorhees et al., 2014). Future studies could therefore benefit from the inclusion of patients with post- traumatic stress disorder as a clinical control group, or a subgroup analysis of BPD patients with and without posttraumatic stress disorder to clarify the contribution of traumatic experiences to aggression in BPD. Kuhlmann et al. (2013) also reported reduced gray matter volume in the cerebellar vermis. Cerebellar structures are connected to the limbic system (Heath & Harper, 1974) and are involved in the regulation of emotional behavior (Sacchetti, Scelfo, & Strata, 2009). Therefore, they might be an additional brain region implicated in affect regulation and aggression.

BPD patients’ aggression has also been reported to be nega- tively associated with gray matter volume in the hippocampus (Sala et al., 2011; Zetzsche et al., 2007), but not in the amygdala (Zetzsche et al., 2006). Comparing male antisocial offenders with BPD to male antisocial offenders with high psychopathic traits, our group found specific structural abnormalities: Male BPD offenders showed smaller volume in brain regions involved in affect regu- lation (orbitofrontal and ventromedial prefrontal cortex), whereas psychopathic offenders showed less volume in cortical midline areas (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and precuneus) that are in- volved in self-referential emotion processing and self-reflection and could thus mirror psychopathic callousness and poor moral judgment (Bertsch, Grothe, et al., 2013).

Taken together, structural neuroimaging studies revealed abnor- malities in gray matter volumes of prefrontal and limbic brain regions in individuals with BPD. These regions are critically implicated in affect dysregulation (Siever, 2008), impulsivity (Coccaro et al., 2011), and threat hypersensitivity (Blair, 2012). Hence, volumetric abnormalities in these regions might be neural corre- lates of aggressive behavior in BPD.

 

Functional Neuroimaging

Consistent with the assumption of prefrontal-limbic imbalance involved in affective dysregulation, functional neuroimaging stud- ies have revealed increased and prolonged amygdala activity in response to (negative) emotional stimuli in BPD patients compared with healthy controls, as well as reduced activity in prefrontal regions involved in regulatory processes such as the anterior cingulate cortex, the medial frontal cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (see, e.g., O’Neill & Frodl, 2012, for a review). In addition, compared with healthy controls, individuals with BPD and comorbid intermittent explosive disor- der (BPD-IED) showed impaired functional connectivity between prefrontal and amygdala regions (New et al., 2007).

These brain structures are also implicated in the regulation of impulsivity (Coccaro et al., 2011). In a positron emission tomog- raphy (PET) study, significant reductions in fluorodeoxyglucose uptake in the bilateral medial orbitofrontal cortex were found in BPD subjects compared with healthy controls. Covarying for mea- sures of impulsivity rendered the differences between groups in- significant (Soloff, Meltzer, et al., 2003). Recent studies suggest a more complex role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the regulation of impulsivity, with reports of differential abnormalities of its subre- gions. The resting state blood flow of BPD patients compared with healthy subjects was reduced in medial regions but increased in lateral regions, and was positively associated with trait impulsivity in both regions (Wolf et al., 2012). Assessing aggression and the brain activity of BPD-IED patients during highly provoking situ- ations in a PET scanner, one study found that experimentally induced provocation led not only to increased aggression in the patients, but also to increased glucose uptake in the orbitofrontal cortex and the amygdala compared with healthy controls (New et al., 2009). In addition, compared with healthy controls, BPD-IED individuals failed to activate anterior, medial, and dorsolateral prefrontal regions during high provocation, suggesting a deficit in the mobilization of higher “cognitive control” of aggression. Whereas the orbitofrontal cortex has often been seen as “put[ting] the brakes on” (New et al., 2009, p. 1112) enhanced affective reactivity of limbic regions such as the amygdala, these results render the interplay more complex. This is in line with investiga- tions in healthy controls which revealed enhanced prefrontal but at the same time reduced orbitofrontal activity during imagined ag- gression (Pietrini, Guazzelli, Basso, Jaffe, & Grafman, 2000). Thus, activations in the orbital frontal cortex may be associated with increased or decreased likelihood of aggressive outbursts depending on social cues and context (Blair, 2004). A second analysis of the data used by New et al. (2009) revealed reduced glucose metabolism rates in the striatum of male BPD-IED pa- tients compared with healthy men and might indicate striatal deficits in providing an appropriate situational context to prefrontal inhibitory areas (Perez-Rodriguez et al., 2012).

Prefrontal-limbic imbalance may additionally underlie BPD pa- tients’ threat hypersensitivity, because their exaggerated initial fixation changes toward the eyes of angry faces were related to increased posterior amygdala reactivity to angry facial expressions (Bertsch, Gamer, et al., 2013).

Functional neuroimaging was also used to investigate neural correlates of BPD patients’ deficits in empathic functioning. Dur- ing tasks assessing cognitive empathy, BPD patients showed re- duced activations in the superior temporal sulcus and the superior temporal gyrus compared with healthy controls (Dziobek et al., 2011; Frick et al., 2012; Mier et al., 2013), whereas they showed enhanced activations in the amygdala (Mier et al., 2013) and insula (Dziobek et al., 2011) during affective empathy. One study also revealed a general hypoactivation in the prefrontal cortex and a hyperactivation in the somatosensory cortex of BPD patients com- pared with healthy controls in a paradigm measuring different facets of empathy (Mier et al., 2013). This suggests that BPD patients have an overactive and poorly controlled amygdala, which might call their unfiltered attention to predominantly negative affective social stimuli. Together with emotional simulation pro- cesses reflected in increased activation of the somatosensory cor- tex, this may lead to emotional contagion, which—as described above—may enhance the likelihood of aggressive reactions.

With regard to the alternative DSM-5 model, the neuroimaging finding of a prefrontal-limbic imbalance in BPD patients underlies the trait facets of emotional lability, impulsivity, and hostility. Hypoactivity of the superior temporal sulcus/gyrus appears to mirror the empathic deficit of BPD patients. Increased activation of the somatosensory cortex can be associated with BPD patients’ tendency to simulate the feelings of others, which may result in what the alternative DSM-5 model conceptualizes as identity dis- turbance. (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 766).

 

Neurochemistry

Aggression and related concepts have been linked to a number of neurochemical substances. This section will begin with a sum- mary of physiological and genetic underpinnings of the serotoner- gic system, followed by other neurochemical systems and their association with aggression in BPD.

Probably the most consistent neurochemical correlate of aggres- sion is a reduced prefrontal serotonergic concentration and func- tion, which has been regarded as a correlate of impulsivity (e.g., Coccaro & Kavoussi, 1997; Siever & Trestman, 1993; Oquendo & Mann, 2000). So far, investigations showed reduced concentra- tions of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of male BPD patients with comorbid intermittent explosive disorder and/or antisocial personality disor- der when compared with clinical controls (Linnoila et al., 1983). 5-Hydroxytryptamine 2a receptor binding abnormalities on plate- lets were positively associated with aggression in a personality- disordered sample, including BPD patients, but not healthy con- trols (Coccaro, Kavoussi, Sheline, Berman, & Csernansky, 1997). Pharmaco-challenge studies that used serotonergic agonists, such as d-fenfluramine, found a deficit in the central serotonergic func- tionality in samples solely with BPD patients (Rinne, Westenberg, den Boer, & van den Brink, 2000; Soloff, Kelly, et al., 2003) or in samples with BPD and other personality disorders (Coccaro et al., 1989; Herpertz, Sass, & Favazza, 1997). Male BPD patients had

diminished serotonergic synthesis capacity, compared with healthy controls (Leyton et al., 2001). Imaging studies located the central serotonergic deficit of BPD patients mainly in prefrontal brain regions (Leyton et al., 2001; Siever et al., 1999; Soloff, Meltzer, et al., 2003; Soloff, Meltzer, Becker, Greer, & Constantine, 2005; Soloff et al., 2000). Compared with healthy controls, BPD patients were also found to show a reduced serotonin transporter density in the cingulate cortex (Frankle et al., 2005) as well as decreased serotonergic functioning in the hypothalamus (Koch et al., 2007) and in hippocampal regions (Soloff et al., 2007).

Heritability accounts for about 50% of aggression (Rhee & Wald- man, 2002) and serotonergic genes; a polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene (Silva et al., 2007) and a haplotype of the tryptophan hydroxylase gene (Perez-Rodriguez et al., 2010) were associated with aggression in BPD patients. However, the identification of a single gene locus with a major effect size on aggression seems unlikely (Craig & Halton, 2009); rather, gene– environment interactions are expected, such as the enhancement of antisocial behavior in subjects with a polymorphism of monoamine oxidase  A  (MAOA) when exposed to childhood maltreatment (Byrd & Manuck, 2014; Caspi et al., 2002). BPD gene– environment studies showed that polymorphisms of the brain-derived growth factor (Wagner, Baskaya, Dahmen, Lieb, & Tadic´, 2010) and the catechol-O-methyltransferase (Wagner, Baskaya, Anicker, et al., 2010) modulated measures of reactive aggression in BPD patients who had experienced severe life events such as physical or sexual maltreatment. However, because of methodological challenges in gene– environment studies (see, e.g., Carpenter, Tomko, Trull, & Boomsma, 2013), the interpretability of these results is limited.

In addition to the serotonin system, several other neurochemicals might play a role in the relationship between BPD and reactive aggression. Oxytocin has been negatively related to reactive aggres- sion by fostering pro-social behavior and secure attachment patterns (see Meyer-Lindenberg, Domes, Kirsch, & Heinrichs, 2011, for a review). In BPD patients, plasma concentrations of oxytocin were negatively associated with trait aggressiveness (Bertsch, Schmidinger, Neumann, & Herpertz, 2013). First findings suggest a beneficial effect of oxytocin on BPD patients’ threat hypersensitivity, as its intranasal administration reduced the speed and number of reflexive eye movements toward the eyes of angry faces and normalized the hyperactivity of the amygdala for angry compared with happy faces in BPD patients (Bertsch, Gamer, et al., 2013). Oxytocin also modulated neuronal activity in brain areas involved in empathic functioning (Domes, Kumbier, Heinrichs, & Herpertz, 2014)—although this has not yet been shown in BPD patients.

Studies suggest that vasopressin might have contrary effects to oxytocin in the modulation of social behavior (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011) and may increase reactive aggression (Haller, 2013). In line with this, the concentration of vasopressin in the CSF has been associated with aggression in personality-disordered subjects, including BPD patients (Coccaro, Kavoussi, Hauger, Cooper, & Ferris, 1998). Additionally, an interrelation between polymor- phisms of the vasopressin receptor gene and reactive aggression in BPD patients has been suggested (Vogel et al., 2012).

Abnormalities in the cortisol system have also been reported in BPD patients, but the results are lacking a clear direction (Zim- merman & Choi-Kain, 2009). Although numerous reports suggest an association between testosterone and aggressive behavior in the general population (Yildirim & Derksen, 2012), findings in BPD patients are sparse. One study reported that CSF concentrations of testosterone did not correlate with previous aggressive behaviors in a sample of male patients with various personality disorders, including BPD (Coccaro, Beresford, Minar, Kaskow, & Geracioti, 2007). Recently, a probable deficit in the opioid system has been gaining increased attention in BPD research (Prossin, Love, Koeppe, Zubieta, & Silk, 2010), and dopamine may also be a factor in the etiology of reactive aggression (Friedel, 2004; Buck- holtz et al., 2010). However, to date, studies investigating these substances in aggression of BPD patients are lacking.

Relating these findings to the alternative DSM-5 model, the central serotonin deficit of BPD patients appears to underlie the trait facet of impulsivity, while there are very preliminary data suggesting that high vasopressinergic and low oxytocinergic ac- tivity might be related to BPD patients’ empathic deficits.

 

Psychophysiology

The most robust psychophysiological correlates of aggression in the general population are as follows: (a) low baseline heart rate, which is regarded as an indicator of the underarousal that predisposes individuals toward compensatory stimulation seeking and thus ag- gression. (b) Enhanced reactivity of heart rate and skin conductance have been linked to aggression, most likely reflecting affective dys- regulation. Regarding further psychophysiological parameters, (c) a reduced P300 amplitude in event-related electroencephalographic po- tentials, although highly dependent on the experimental context, has been associated with impulsivity and aggression, (d) more slow wave activity in resting electroencephalography has also been thought to reflect impulsivity, and (e) an augmented potentiation of the startle eyeblink response to aversive stimuli has been associated with affec- tive dysregulation (Lorber, 2004; Patrick, 2008).

In line with this, BPD patients, when compared with healthy controls, showed an enhanced reactivity of heart rate (Ebner- Priemer et al., 2007) and skin conductance (Barnow et al., 2012), more slow wave activity in resting electroencephalography (Sny- der & Pitts, 1984), and augmented potentiation of the startle response (Ebner-Priemer et al., 2005; Hazlett et al., 2007). Com- pared with clinical controls, BPD patients demonstrated dimin- ished P300 amplitude in a 2-tone discrimination task (Blackwood, St Clair, & Kutcher, 1986). The results of a very recent event- related potential study by our group additionally showed enhanced early visual potentials (occipital P100) in BPD patients compared with healthy controls in a facial recognition task with blends of happy versus angry expressions (Izurieta, Nagy, Mancke, Herpertz, & Bertsch, 2014), indicating an involvement of very early visual processes in threat hypersensitivity.

With regard to the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD, psy- chophysiological data most notably reflect the trait facets of emotional lability (enhanced skin conductance, heart rate reac- tivity and augmented startle response) and impulsivity (in-  creased slow wave activity and reduced P300 amplitude). En-  hanced P100 amplitudes probably point to early attentional hypervigilance to threat-related social cues, favoring hostility. These studies, however, have rarely included measures of ag- gression, making implications for the manifestation of aggres- sive behaviors in BPD premature.

Multidimensional Model of Aggression in BPD

In this concluding section, we will use the reviewed data to propose a multidimensional model of aggression in BPD. We summarize findings of aggression in BPD from the perspective of the biobehav- ioral dimensions affective dysregulation, impulsivity, threat hypersen- sitivity, and empathic functioning; and then link these findings to the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD (see also Figure 1).

Based on this approach, affective dysregulation, which has been shown to be associated with prefrontal-limbic imbalance, enhanced reactivity of heart rate, skin conductance, and startle response, is close to the trait facet of emotional lability in the alternative DSM-5 model. Impulsivity is also associated with prefrontal-limbic imbalance as well as central serotonergic dysfunction, more electroencephalographic slow wave activity and reduced P300 amplitude in a 2-tone discrim- ination task, and is directly addressed as a trait facet in DSM-5. The biobehavioral dimension of threat hypersensitivity reflected in en- hanced perception of anger in ambiguous facial expressions, greater speed and number of reflexive eye movements to angry eyes, prefrontal-limbic imbalance, and enhanced P100 amplitude in re- sponse to blends of happy versus angry facial expressions can be linked to the DSM-5 trait facets of emotional lability and hostility. Additionally, threat hypersensitivity is related to impairments in in- terpersonal functioning, more specifically to interpersonal hypersen- sitivity and a negatively biased perception of others, which might be modulated by oxytocin. Reduced cognitive empathy was shown to be associated with hypoactivity in the superior temporal sulcus/gyrus and is encompassed in the description of the empathic deficit of DSM-5. Preliminary data suggest an additional role for lower oxytocinergic and higher vasopressinergic activity in reduced cognitive empathy. Finally, reduced self– other differentiation coupled with greater emo- tional simulation and hyperactivation of the somatosensory cortex might contribute to BPD patients’ identity disturbance. Notably, these various biobehavioral dimensions may contribute to aggression to different extents within the individual BPD patient.

Importantly, we suppose that the biobehavioral dimensions are highly interconnected. This is demonstrated by several findings. The serotonergic dysfunction is related not only to impulsivity but also to affective dysregulation (Canli & Lesch, 2007; Davidson et al., 2000), and the brain regions implicated in affective dysregulation are also linked to impulsivity (Goodman & New, 2000), empathic functioning (Adolphs, 2009) and threat hypersensitivity (Blair, 2012). Affective dysregulation mediates the emergence not only of threat hyper- sensitivity (Gratz, Dixon-Gordon, Breetz, & Tull, 2013) but also of deficits in empathic functioning (Sharp et al., 2011). Finally, within the framework of the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD, hostility is a trait facet that has been subsumed under both pathological personality domains, antagonism and negative af- fectivity (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 780).

 

Limitations

Interestingly, the biobehavioral dimensions shown to underlie ag- gression in BPD can be nicely related to the terminology and defini- tions of the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD according to Section III. However, some inconsistencies have to be discussed. In the alterna- tive DSM-5 model, interpersonal hypersensitivity is related to the empathic deficit, whereas reduced self– other differentiation, a facet of empathy in concepts of neuroscience (Decety & Jackson, 2004), is subsumed under the identity disturbance. Although this primarily seems reasonable to us, interpersonal hypersensitivity also encom- passes aspects that are only partially embraced by the concept of empathy in DSM-5, but also overlaps with descriptions of trait facets, for example, by partially overlapping with hostility, and closely in- teracting with emotional lability and impulsivity in the context of escalating transactional processes. Consequently, the conceptual boundaries between the impaired personality functioning and the pathological personality traits are not clear-cut and are most likely to be intertwined, as already discussed by Livesley (2012) and, thus, need to be further developed and conceptually sharpened in the future. However, the dimensional approach of the alternative DSM-5 model seems to explain the complex multifaceted behavioral con- struct of aggression better than a categorical approach to BPD, which has raised questions such as whether aggression is, in fact, an inherent characteristic of the disorder, or a subtype of BPD (Hallquist & Pilkonis, 2012; Kernberg & Caligor, 2005), or rather a co-occurring phenomenon related to comorbid personality pa- thology, such as antisocial personality disorder or psychopathy (Allen & Links, 2012). The latter assumption seemed to be con- firmed by the nonsignificant relationship between aggression and BPD after statistical controlling for Axis I or other comorbid Cluster B personality disorders (Berman, Fallon, & Coccaro, 1998; Haller & Miles, 2003; Johnson et al., 2000). However, because almost two thirds (66%) of BPD patients who do not fulfill the criteria for antisocial personality disorder still engage in aggres- sion (Newhill et al., 2009), aggression is more likely to be linked to BPD psychopathology itself, and further investigations of (neu- ro)biological and psychological correlates of reactive aggression in patients with BPD are highly important.

The specificity of some of the data presented above requires further exploration. For instance, associations between prefrontal dysfunction and aggression have been reported in a variety of psychiatric conditions (Bassarath, 2001; Brower & Price, 2001) and the same holds true for alterations in the serotonin system (Bortolato et al., 2013), again questioning a purely categorical approach.

It should be considered that the range of effect sizes in the above-presented data is immense: from as little as 5% shared variance for BPD traits and reactive aggression (Gardner et al., 2012) to >57% mediation of aggression by interpersonal problems in individuals with BPD traits (Stepp et al., 2012).

Finally, the aim of this article was not to completely reconcep- tualize the relationship between aggression and BPD or to com- prehensively review all available data. We rather intended to provide a specific framework for aggression in BPD that poses unanswered questions and stimulates further discussion and integrative research.

 

Figure 1. Multidimensional model of reactive aggression in borderline personality disorder (BPD). As depicted in the figure legend (right bottom), the circles represent the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD, the biobehavioral dimensions, as well as the data from behavioral tasks, psychophysiology, neuroimaging, and neurochemistry. Based on this approach reactive aggression in BPD is conceptualized in terms of the biobehavioral dimensions of affective dysregulation, impulsivity, threat hypersensitivity, reduced cognitive empathy, and reduced self-other differentiation, which in turn can be linked to certain aspects of the alternative DSM-5 model of BPD. For instance, affective dysregulation is associated with enhanced reactivity of heart rate, skin conductance, startle response, and prefrontal-limbic imbalance, and can be linked to the trait facet of emotional lability. The biobehavioral dimensions are highly interconnected. For instance, prefrontal-limbic imbalance is associated with affective dysregulation, impulsivity as well as threat hypersensitivty, and emotional lability can be linked to affective dysregulation and threat hypersensitivity. Act. = activity, Ampl. = amplitude, Dis. = discrimination, Diff. = differentiation, EEG = electroencephalographic, Sup. = superior, Temp. = temporal.

 

 

 

Outlook

Pathological forms of aggression are an individual as well as a societal burden that remain far from being understood (World Health Organization, 2007). With regard to aggression in BPD, a number of questions need to be addressed in future research.

First, the construct validity, that is the accuracy of the existing paradigms for the study of reactive aggression in BPD (Giancola & Chermack, 1998, p. 238), has been questioned. The restriction to stimulus–response sequences as conducted in most of the experi- mental paradigms leaves out underlying motives and intentions of aggressive behavior (Tedeschi & Quigley, 2000) and the relation- ship between provocateur and aggressor that seems to be of par- ticular importance in BPD. Thus, instruments that mimic aggres- sion in an ecologically valid manner are urgently needed.

Second, aggression has been shown to be modulated by disso- ciation (Moskowitz, 2004), depressiveness (Bjork, Dougherty, & Moeller, 1997; Dougherty et al., 2004), risk-taking (Zuckerman & Kuhlman, 2000), and low self-esteem (Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins, Moffitt, & Caspi, 2005; cf. Bushman et al., 2009), all of which are important features of BPD psychopathology. Hence, future studies should focus on their specific impact on aggression in BPD.

Third, the reliability and precision of neurobiological data cor- respond to the technical standards presently available. For exam-

ple, the direction of the neurochemical alterations, their exact localizations within the human brain, and the interplay between different substances remain unclear. In the future, innovative neu- roimaging techniques will elucidate smaller and more definite anatomical structures, such as orbitofrontal subregions and amygdala subnuclei, and connectivities within circuits that may help to identify more specific mechanisms underlying trait dimen- sions and categorical entities.

Fourth, gender differences in aggression are well known in the general population (Archer, 2004; Campbell, 2006), but still need to be addressed in BPD, as most previous studies concentrated on female subjects.

Last but not least, therapeutic options for aggression in BPD are limited (Látalová & Prasko, 2010). Psychotherapeutic strategies, which are said to be the most effective treatment for BPD (Stoffers et al., 2012), need to be adapted to the particular needs of patients with aggressive behavior. First results in this regard are promising, especially for dialectical behavioral therapy (Evershed et al., 2003; Frazier & Vela, 2014; Nelson-Gray et al., 2006; Shelton, Kesten, Zhang, & Trestman, 2011). In terms of biological treatments, the search for and investigation of new agents, for instance oxytocin agonists or vasopressin antagonists, could be promising.

In conclusion, the investigation and classification of aggression in BPD requires a multidimensional approach. In our opinion, the alternative DSM-5 model can be used as a framework to concep- tualize BPD-associated aggression and its underlying biobehav- ioral alterations. Further research pursuing a multimethod ap- proach on the behavioral and the pathophysiological level is needed to explicate the fundamental biobehavioral dimensions in the field of personality disorders. Analogously to research in the major mental disorders (Insel, 2014), these dimensions are signif- icant for the understanding of etiology and pathophysiology of BPD, and should facilitate the development of new treatments for those affected.

 

References

Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693–716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/ annurev.psych.60.110707.163514

Allen, A., & Links, P. S. (2012). Aggression in borderline personality disorder: Evidence for increased risk and clinical predictors. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14, 62– 69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11920-011-0244-9

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man- ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231

Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology, 8, 291–322. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291

Arntz, A. (1994). Treatment of borderline personality disorder: A challenge for cognitive-behavioural therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 419 – 430. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)90005-1

Arntz, A., Dreessen, L., Schouten, E., & Weertman, A. (2004). Beliefs in personality disorders: A test with the personality disorder belief ques- tionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1215–1225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.004

Barnow, S., Limberg, A., Stopsack, M., Spitzer, C., Grabe, H. J., Frey- berger, H. J., & Hamm, A. (2012). Dissociation and emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 42, 783– 794. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001917

Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 38, 813– 822. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x

Barratt, E. S., & Felthous, A. R. (2003). Impulsive versus premeditated aggression: Implications for mens rea decisions. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 21, 619 – 630. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bsl.555

Bassarath, L. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of antisocial behaviour. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie, 46, 728 –732.

Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Berkowitz, L. (2003). Affect, aggression and antisocial behavior. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 804 – 823). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Berman, M. E., Fallon, A. E., & Coccaro, E. F. (1998). The relationship between personality psychopathology and aggressive behavior in re- search volunteers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 651– 658. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.107.4.651

Bernstein, D., & Fink, L. (1998). Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. A retrospective self-report manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Bertsch, K., Gamer, M., Schmidt, B., Schmidinger, I., Walther, S., Kästel, T., . . . Herpertz, S. C. (2013). Oxytocin and reduction of social threat hypersensitivity in women with borderline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 1169 –1177. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13020263

Bertsch, K., Grothe, M., Prehn, K., Vohs, K., Berger, C., Hauenstein, K.,

. . . Herpertz, S. C. (2013). Brain volumes differ between diagnostic groups of violent criminal offenders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, 593– 606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s00406-013-0391-6

Bertsch, K., Schmidinger, I., Neumann, I. D., & Herpertz, S. C. (2013). Reduced plasma oxytocin levels in female patients with borderline personality disorder. Hormones and Behavior, 63, 424 – 429. http://dx

.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.013

Bjork, J. M., Dougherty, D. M., & Moeller, F. G. (1997). A positive correlation between self-ratings of depression and laboratory-measured aggression. Psychiatry Research, 69, 33–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0165-1781(96)03025-9

Black, D. W., Gunter, T., Allen, J., Blum, N., Arndt, S., Wenman, G., & Sieleni, B. (2007). Borderline personality disorder in male and female offenders newly committed to prison. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48, 400 – 405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.04.006

Blackwood, D. H., St. Clair, D. M., & Kutcher, S. P. (1986). P300 event-related potential abnormalities in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 21, 560 –564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(86)90202-7

Blair, R. J. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain and Cognition, 55, 198 –208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00276-8

Blair, R. J. (2012). Considering anger from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3, 65– 74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.154

Blair, R. J. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 786 –799. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ nrn3577

Blair, R. J., Peschardt, K. S., Budhani, S., Mitchell, D. G. V., & Pine, D. S. (2006). The development of psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 47(3– 4), 262–276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01596.x

Blanchard, R. J., Blanchard, D. C., Takahashi, T., & Kelley, M. J. (1977). Attack and defensive behaviour in the albino rat. Animal Behaviour, 25, 622– 634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(77)90113-0

Bortolato, M., Pivac, N., Muck Seler, D., Nikolac Perkovic, M., Pessia, M., & Di Giovanni, G. (2013). The role of the serotonergic system at the interface of aggression and suicide. Neuroscience, 236, 160 –185. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.015

Brower, M. C., & Price, B. H. (2001). Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: A critical review. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 71, 720 –726. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.71.6.720

Brunner, R., Henze, R., Parzer, P., Kramer, J., Feigl, N., Lutz, K., . . . Stieltjes, B. (2010). Reduced prefrontal and orbitofrontal gray matter in female adolescents with borderline personality disorder: Is it disorder specific? NeuroImage, 49, 114 –120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.070

Buckholtz, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Cowan, R. L., Woodward, N. D., Benning, S. D., Li, R., . . . Zald, D. H. (2010). Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 419 – 421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2510

Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. F., Thomaes, S., Ryu, E., Begeer, S., & West, S. G. (2009). Looking again, and harder, for a link between low self-esteem and aggression. Journal of Personality, 77, 427– 446. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00553.x

Byrd, A. L., & Manuck, S. B. (2014). MAOA, childhood maltreatment, and antisocial behavior: Meta-analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 9 –17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.004

Campbell, A. (2006). Sex differences in direct aggression: What are the psychological mediators? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 237– 264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2005.09.002

Canli, T., & Lesch, K.-P. (2007). Long story short: The serotonin trans- porter in emotion regulation and social cognition. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1103–1109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1964

Carpenter, R. W., Tomko, R. L., Trull, T. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2013). Gene-environment studies and borderline personality disorder: A re- view. Current Psychiatry Reports, 15, 336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s11920-012-0336-1

Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., . . . Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal- treated children. Science, 297, 851– 854. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/ science.1072290

Chanen, A. M., Velakoulis, D., Carison, K., Gaunson, K., Wood, S. J., Yuen, H. P., . . . Pantelis, C. (2008). Orbitofrontal, amygdala and hippocampal volumes in teenagers with first-presentation borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 163, 116 –125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.08.007

Coccaro, E. F., Beresford, B., Minar, P., Kaskow, J., & Geracioti, T. (2007). CSF testosterone: Relationship to aggression, impulsivity, and venturesomeness in adult males with personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41, 488 – 492. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.04.009

Coccaro, E. F., & Kavoussi, R. J. (1997). Fluoxetine and impulsive aggressive behavior in personality-disordered subjects. Archives of Gen- eral Psychiatry, 54, 1081–1088. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsych.1997.01830240035005

Coccaro, E. F., Kavoussi, R. J., Hauger, R. L., Cooper, T. B., & Ferris,

  1. F. (1998). Cerebrospinal fluid vasopressin levels: Correlates with aggression and serotonin  function  in  personality-disordered subjects.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 708 –714. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.55.8.708

Coccaro, E. F., Kavoussi, R. J., Sheline, Y. I., Berman, M. E., & Csern- ansky, J. G. (1997). Impulsive aggression in personality disorder corre- lates with platelet 5-HT2A receptor binding. Neuropsychopharmacol- ogy, 16, 211–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00194-7

Coccaro, E. F., Siever, L. J., Klar, H. M., Maurer, G., Cochrane, K., Cooper, T. B., . . . Davis, K. L. (1989). Serotonergic studies in patients with affective and personality disorders. Correlates with suicidal and impulsive aggressive behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 587–599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810070013002

Coccaro, E. F., Sripada, C. S., Yanowitch, R. N., & Phan, K. L. (2011). Corticolimbic function in impulsive aggressive behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 1153–1159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.032

Costa, D. M., & Babcock, J. C. (2008). Articulated thoughts of intimate partner abusive men during anger arousal: Correlates with personality disorder features. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 395– 402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-008-9163-x

Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five Factor Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psycho- logical Assessment Resources.

Craig, I. W., & Halton, K. E. (2009). Genetics of human aggressive behaviour. Human Genetics, 126, 101–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s00439-009-0695-9

Critchfield, K. L., Levy, K. N., & Clarkin, J. F. (2004). The relationship between impulsivity, aggression, and impulsive-aggression in borderline personality disorder: An empirical analysis of self-report measures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18, 555–570. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1521/pedi.18.6.555.54795

Critchfield, K. L., Levy, K. N., Clarkin, J. F., & Kernberg, O. F. (2008). The relational context of aggression in borderline personality disorder: Using adult attachment style to predict forms of hostility. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 67– 82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20434

Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation—A possible prelude to violence. Science, 289, 591–594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.289.5479.591 Decety, J. (2011). The neuroevolution of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1231, 35– 45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06027.x

Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187

de Waal, F. B. M. (2012). The antiquity of empathy. Science, 336, 874 – 876. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1220999

Domes, G., Czieschnek, D., Weidler, F., Berger, C., Fast, K., & Herpertz,

  1. C. (2008). Recognition of facial affect in Borderline Personality Disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22, 135–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2008.22.2.135

Domes, G., Kumbier, E., Heinrichs, M., & Herpertz, S. C. (2014). Oxy- tocin promotes facial emotion recognition and amygdala reactivity in adults with Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 698 – 706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.254

Domes, G., Schulze, L., & Herpertz, S. C. (2009). Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder-a review of the literature. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 6 –19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.6

Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2005). Low self-esteem is related to aggression, antisocial behavior, and delinquency. Psychological Science, 16, 328 –335. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01535.x

Dougherty, D. M., Bjork, J. M., Huckabee, H. C., Moeller, F. G., & Swann,

  1. C. (1999). Laboratory measures of aggression and impulsivity in

women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 85,

315–326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(99)00011-6

Dougherty, D. D., Rauch, S. L., Deckersbach, T., Marci, C., Loh, R., Shin,

  1. , . . . Fava, M. (2004). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala dysfunction during an anger induction positron emission to- mography study in patients with major depressive disorder with anger attacks. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 795– 804. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.795

Dziobek, I., Preissler, S., Grozdanovic, Z., Heuser, I., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S. (2011). Neuronal correlates of altered empathy and social cognition in borderline personality disorder. NeuroImage, 57, 539 –548. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.005

Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Badeck, S., Beckmann, C., Wagner, A., Feige, B., Weiss, I.,… Bohus, M. (2005). Affective dysregulation and dissociative experience in female patients with borderline personality disorder: A startle response study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39, 85–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.05.001

Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Welch, S. S., Grossman, P., Reisch, T., Linehan,

  1. , & Bohus, M. (2007). Psychophysiological ambulatory assess- ment of affective dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Psy- chiatry Research, 150, 265–275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.014

Evershed, S., Tennant, A., Boomer, D., Rees, A., Barkham, M., & Watson,

  1. (2003). Practice-based outcomes of dialectical behaviour therapy (DBT) targeting anger and violence, with male forensic patients: A pragmatic and non-contemporaneous comparison. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 13, 198 –213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cbm.542

Fertuck, E. A., Jekal, A., Song, I., Wyman, B., Morris, M. C., Wilson,

  1. T., . . . Stanley, B. (2009). Enhanced ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ in borderline personality disorder compared to healthy controls. Psycho- logical Medicine, 39, 1979 –1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ S003329170900600X

Fonagy, P. (2003). Towards a developmental understanding of violence. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 183, 190 –192. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1192/bjp.183.3.190

Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2009). A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 1355–1381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409990198

Fossati, A., Acquarini, E., Feeney, J. A., Borroni, S., Grazioli, F., Giarolli,

  1. E., . . . Maffei, C. (2009). Alexithymia and attachment insecurities in impulsive aggression. Attachment & Human Development, 11, 165–182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14616730802625235

Fossati, A., Barratt, E. S., Carretta, I., Leonardi, B., Grazioli, F., & Maffei,

  1. (2004). Predicting borderline and antisocial personality disorder features in nonclinical subjects using measures of impulsivity and ag- gressiveness. Psychiatry Research, 125, 161–170. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.12.001

Frankle, W. G., Lombardo, I., New, A. S., Goodman, M., Talbot, P. S., Huang, Y., . . . Siever, L. J. (2005). Brain serotonin transporter distri- bution in subjects with impulsive aggressivity: A positron emission study with [11C]McN 5652. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 915–923. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.915

Frazier, S. N., & Vela, J. (2014). Dialectical behavior therapy for the treatment of anger and aggressive behavior: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19, 156 –163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2014.02.001

Frick, C., Lang, S., Kotchoubey, B., Sieswerda, S., Dinu-Biringer, R., Berger, M., . . . Barnow, S. (2012). Hypersensitivity in borderline personality disorder during mindreading. PLoS ONE, 7, e41650. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041650

Friedel, R. O. (2004). Dopamine dysfunction in borderline personality disorder: A hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 1029 –1039. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300424

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5

García-Forero, C., Gallardo-Pujol, D., Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Andrés- Pueyo, A. (2009). Disentangling impulsiveness, aggressiveness and im- pulsive aggression: An empirical approach using self-report measures. Psychiatry Research, 168, 40 – 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.04.002

Gardner, D. L., Leibenluft, E., O’Leary, K. M., & Cowdry, R. W. (1991). Self-ratings of anger and hostility in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 157–161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199103000-00008

Gardner, K. J., Archer, J., & Jackson, S. (2012). Does maladaptive coping mediate the relationship between borderline personality traits and reac- tive and proactive aggression? Aggressive Behavior, 38, 403– 413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.21437

Giancola, P. R., & Chermack, S. T. (1998). Construct validity of laboratory aggression paradigms: A response to Tedeschi and Quigly (1996). Ag- gression and Violent Behavior, 3, 237–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(97)00004-9

Gollan, J. K., Lee, R., & Coccaro, E. F. (2005). Developmental psycho- pathology and neurobiology of aggression. Development and Psychopa- thology, 17, 1151–1171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050546 Goodman, M., & New, A. (2000). Impulsive aggression in borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 2, 56 – 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11920-000-0043-1

Gratz, K. L., Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Breetz, A., & Tull, M. (2013). A laboratory-based examination of responses to social rejection in border- line personality disorder: The mediating role of emotion dysregulation. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27, 157–171. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.2.157

Gregg, T. R., & Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: Implications for human aggression. Prog- ress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 25, 91– 140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00150-0

Gunderson, J. G. (1996). The borderline patient’s intolerance of aloneness: Insecure attachments and therapist availability. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 752–758.

Gunderson, J. G., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2008). BPD’s interpersonal hyper- sensitivity phenotype: A gene-environment-developmental model. Jour- nal of Personality Disorders, 22, 22– 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2008.22.1.22

Haller, D. L., & Miles, D. R. (2003). Victimization and perpetration among perinatal substance abusers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 760 – 780. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260503253239

Haller, J. (2013). The neurobiology of abnormal manifestations of aggres- sion—A review of hypothalamic mechanisms in cats, rodents, and humans. Brain Research Bulletin, 93, 97–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.brainresbull.2012.10.003

Hallquist, M. N., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2012). Refining the phenotype of borderline personality disorder: Diagnostic criteria and beyond. Person- ality Disorders, 3, 228 –246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027953

Harari, H., Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Ravid, M., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in border- line personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 175, 277–279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.03.002

Hazlett, E. A., New, A. S., Newmark, R., Haznedar, M. M., Lo, J. N., Speiser, L. J., . . . Buchsbaum, M. S. (2005). Reduced anterior and posterior cingulate gray matter in borderline personality disorder. Bio- logical Psychiatry, 58, 614 – 623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.029

Hazlett, E. A., Speiser, L. J., Goodman, M., Roy, M., Carrizal, M., Wynn, K.,… New, A. S. (2007). Exaggerated affect-modulated startle during unpleasant stimuli in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychi- atry, 62, 250 –255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.028

Heath, R. G., & Harper, J. W. (1974). Ascending projections of the cerebellar fastigial nucleus to the hippocampus, amygdala, and other temporal lobe sites: Evoked potential and histological studies in mon- keys and cats. Experimental Neurology, 45, 268 –287. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0014-4886(74)90118-6

Herpertz, S., Sass, H., & Favazza, A. (1997). Impulsivity in self-mutilative behavior: Psychometric and biological findings. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 31, 451– 465. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3956(97)00004-6

Herpertz, S. C., Werth, U., Lukas, G., Qunaibi, M., Schuerkens, A., Kunert, J., . . . Sass, H. (2001). Emotion in criminal offenders with psychop- athy and borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychia- try, 58, 737–745. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.8.737

Herr, N. R., Keenan-Miller, D., Rosenthal, M. Z., & Feldblum, J. (2013). Negative interpersonal events mediate the relation between borderline features and aggressive behavior: Findings from a nonclinical sample of undergraduate women. Personality Disorders, 4, 254 –260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032212

Hines, D. (2008). Borderline personality traits and intimate partner aggres- sion: An international multisite, cross-gender analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 290 –302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00437.x

Hollander, E., Swann, A. C., Coccaro, E. F., Jiang, P., & Smith, T. B. (2005). Impact of trait impulsivity and state aggression on divalproex versus placebo response in borderline personality disorder. The Ameri- can Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 621– 624. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.621

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Meehan, J. C., Herron, K., Rehman, U., & Stuart, L. (2000). Testing the Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994) batterer typology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 1000 – 1019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.6.1000

Insel, T. R. (2014). The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Project: Precision medicine for psychiatry. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 395–397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020138

Izurieta, N., Nagy, K., Mancke, F., Herpertz, S. C., & Bertsch, K. (2014). Time course of facial emotion processing in borderline personality disorder—An ERP study. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Jeung, H., & Herpertz, S. C. (2014). Impairments of interpersonal func- tioning: Empathy and intimacy in borderline personality disorder. Psy- chopathology, 47, 220 –234.

Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes, E., Kasen, S., Oldham, J. M., Skodol, E., & Brook, J. S. (2000). Adolescent personality disorders associated with violence and criminal behavior during adolescence and early adult- hood. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1406 –1412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.9.1406

Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A sys- tematic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 441– 476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2003.03.001

Kernberg, O., & Caligor, E. (2005). A psychoanalytic theory of personality disorders. In M. Lenzenweger & J. Clarkin (Eds.), Major theories of personality disorder (2nd ed., pp. 115–156). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Koch, W., Schaaff, N., Pöpperl, G., Mulert, C., Juckel, G., Reicherzer, M.,. . . Pogarell, O. (2007). [I-123] ADAM and SPECT in patients with borderline personality disorder and healthy control subjects. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 32, 234 –240.

Koenigsberg, H. W., Harvey, P. D., Mitropoulou, V., New, A. S., Good- man, M., Silverman, J., . . . Siever, L. J. (2001). Are the interpersonal and identity disturbances in the borderline personality disorder criteria linked to the traits of affective instability and impulsivity? Journal of Personality Disorders, 15, 358 –370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.15.4.358.19181

Kuhlmann, A., Bertsch, K., Schmidinger, I., Thomann, P. A., & Herpertz, C. (2013). Morphometric differences in central stress-regulating struc- tures between women with and without borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 38, 129 –137. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1503/jpn.120039

Látalová, K., & Prasko, J. (2010). Aggression in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatric Quarterly, 81, 239 –251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s11126-010-9133-3

Leyton, M., Okazawa, H., Diksic, M., Paris, J., Rosa, P., Mzengeza, S.,… Benkelfat, C. (2001). Brain Regional alpha-[11C]methyl-L-tryptophan trapping in impulsive subjects with borderline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 775–782. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.5.775

Linnoila, M., Virkkunen, M., Scheinin, M., Nuutila, A., Rimon, R., & Goodwin, F. K. (1983). Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Sciences, 33, 2609 –2614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(83)90344-2

Livesley, J. (2012). Tradition versus empiricism in the current DSM-5 proposal for revising the classification of personality disorders. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 22, 81–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cbm.1826

Lorber, M. F. (2004). Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 531– 552. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.531

Matzke, B., Herpertz, S. C., Berger, C., Fleischer, M., & Domes, G. (2014). Facial reactions during emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: A facial electromyography study. Psychopathology, 47, 101– 110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000351122

McCloskey, M. S., New, A. S., Siever, L. J., Goodman, M., Koenigsberg, W., Flory, J. D., & Coccaro, E. F. (2009). Evaluation of behavioral impulsivity and aggression tasks as endophenotypes for borderline per- sonality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 1036 –1048. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.01.002

Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Domes, G., Kirsch, P., & Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: Social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 524 –538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3044

Mier, D., Lis, S., Esslinger, C., Sauer, C., Hagenhoff, M., Ulferts, J., . . . Kirsch, P. (2013). Neuronal correlates of social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 531–537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss028

Minzenberg, M. J., Fan, J., New, A. S., Tang, C. Y., & Siever, L. J. (2008). Frontolimbic structural changes in borderline personality disorder. Jour- nal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 727–733. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j

.jpsychires.2007.07.015

Morandotti, N., Dima, D., Jogia, J., Frangou, S., Sala, M., Vidovich, G. Z., . . . Brambilla, P. (2013). Childhood abuse is associated with structural impairment in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and aggressiveness in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neu- roimaging, 213, 18 –23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.02.002

Moskowitz, A. (2004). Dissociation and violence: A review of the litera- ture. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 5, 21– 46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524838003259321

Nelson-Gray, R. O., Keane, S. P., Hurst, R. M., Mitchell, J. T., Warburton,B., Chok, J. T., & Cobb, A. R. (2006). A modified DBT skills training program for oppositional defiant adolescents: Promising preliminary findings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1811–1820. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.01.004

New, A. S., Aan het Rot, M., Ripoll, L. H., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Lazarus, S., Zipursky, E., . . . Siever, L. J. (2012). Empathy and alexithymia in borderline personality disorder: Clinical and laboratory measures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26, 660 – 675. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2012.26.5.660

New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A., Buchsbaum, M. S., Goodman, M., Mitelman, A., Newmark, R., . . . Siever, L. J. (2007). Amygdala-prefrontal disconnection in borderline personality disorder. Neuropsychopharma- cology, 32, 1629 –1640. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301283

New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A., Newmark, R. E., Zhang, J., Triebwasser, J., Meyerson, D., . . . Buchsbaum, M. S. (2009). Laboratory induced aggression: A positron emission tomography study of aggressive indi- viduals with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 1107–1114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.015

Newhill, C. E., Eack, S. M., & Mulvey, E. P. (2009). Violent behavior in borderline personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 541–554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.6.541

Newhill, C. E., Eack, S. M., & Mulvey, E. P. (2012). A growth curve analysis of emotion dysregulation as a mediator for violence in individ- uals with and without borderline personality disorder. Journal of Per- sonality Disorders, 26, 452– 467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2012.26.3.452

Nunes, P. M., Wenzel, A., Borges, K. T., Porto, C. R., Caminha, R. M., & de Oliveira, I. R. (2009). Volumes of the hippocampus and amygdala in patients with borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 333–345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.333

O’Neill, A., & Frodl, T. (2012). Brain structure and function in borderline personality disorder. Brain Structure & Function, 217, 767–782. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0379-4

Oquendo, M. A., & Mann, J. J. (2000). The biology of impulsivity and suicidality. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23, 11–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70140-4

Ostrov, J. M., & Houston, R. J. (2008). The utility of forms and functions of aggression in emerging adulthood: Association with personality dis- order symptomatology. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 1147– 1158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9289-4

Patrick, C. J. (2008). Psychophysiological correlates of aggression and violence: An integrative review. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 363, 2543–2555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0028

Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Hazlett, E. A., Rich, E. L., Ripoll, L. H., Weiner, M., Spence, N., . . . New, A. S. (2012). Striatal activity in borderline personality disorder with comorbid intermittent explosive disorder: Sex differences. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46, 797– 804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.02.014

Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Weinstein, S., New, A. S., Bevilacqua, L., Yuan, Q., Zhou, Z., . . . Siever, L. J. (2010). Tryptophan-hydroxylase 2 haplotype association with borderline personality disorder and aggres- sion in a sample of patients with personality disorders and healthy controls. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44, 1075–1081. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.014

Peters, J. R., Geiger, P. J., Smart, L. M., & Baer, R. A. (2014). Shame and borderline personality features: The potential mediating role of anger and anger rumination. Personality Disorders, 5, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/per0000022

Pietrini, P., Guazzelli, M., Basso, G., Jaffe, K., & Grafman, J. (2000). Neural correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior assessed by positron emission tomography in healthy subjects. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1772–1781. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1772

Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 12, 115– 122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.115

Preißler, S., Dziobek, I., Ritter, K., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S. (2010). Social cognition in borderline personality disorder: Evidence for dis-turbed recognition of the emotions, thoughts, and intentions of others. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 182.

Pretzer, J. (1990). Borderline Personality disorder. In A. T. Beck & A. Freeman (Eds.), Cognitive therapy of personality disorder (pp. 176 – 207). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Prossin, A. R., Love, T. M., Koeppe, R. A., Zubieta, J. K., & Silk, K. R. (2010). Dysregulation of regional endogenous opioid function in bor- derline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 925–933. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091348

Raine, A. (1993). Features of borderline personality and violence. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 277–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097- 4679(199303)49:2<277::AID-JCLP2270490222>3.0.CO;2-C

Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Séguin, J. R., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G.,… Pérusse, D. (2010). Interactive links between theory of mind, peer victimization, and reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Abnor- mal Child Psychology, 38, 1109 –1123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s10802-010-9432-z

Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influ- ences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490 –529. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1037/0033-2909.128.3.490

Rinne, T., Westenberg, H. G., den Boer, J. A., & van den Brink, W. (2000). Serotonergic blunting to meta-chlorophenylpiperazine (m-CPP) highly correlates with sustained childhood abuse in impulsive and autoaggres- sive female borderline patients. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 548 –556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00181-X

Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preissler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., . . . Roepke, S. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic per- sonality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 187, 241–247. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.013

Ross, J. M., & Babcock, J. C. (2009). Proactive and reactive violence among intimate partner violent men diagnosed with antisocial and bor- derline personality disorder. Journal of Family Violence, 24, 607– 617. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-009-9259-y

Rüsch, N., Lieb, K., Göttler, I., Hermann, C., Schramm, E., Richter, H.,… Bohus, M. (2007). Shame and implicit self-concept in women with borderline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 500 –508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.164.3.500

Sacchetti, B., Scelfo, B., & Strata, P. (2009). Cerebellum and emotional behavior. Neuroscience, 162, 756 –762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.064

Sala, M., Caverzasi, E., Lazzaretti, M., Morandotti, N., De Vidovich, G., Marraffini, E., . . . Brambilla, P. (2011). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and hippocampus sustain impulsivity and aggressiveness in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 131(1–3), 417– 421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.036

Schmahl, C. G., & Herpertz, S. (2014). Clinical promise of translational research in borderline personality disorder. In J. Oldham, A. Skodol, & Bender (Eds.), Textbook of personality disorders. Arlington, VA: The American Psychiatric Publishing.

Scott, L. N., Stepp, S. D., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2014). Prospective associa- tions between features of borderline personality disorder, emotion dys- regulation, and aggression. Personality Disorders, 5, 278 –288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000070

Sharp, C., Pane, H., Ha, C., Venta, A., Patel, A. B., Sturek, J., & Fonagy, (2011). Theory of mind and emotion regulation difficulties in ado- lescents with borderline traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 563–573.e1. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.017

Shelton, D., Kesten, K., Zhang, W., & Trestman, R. (2011). Impact of a Dialectic Behavior Therapy-Corrections Modified (DBT-CM) upon be- haviorally challenged incarcerated male adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 24, 105–113.

Siever, L. J. (2008). Neurobiology of aggression and violence. The Amer- ican Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 429 – 442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ appi.ajp.2008.07111774

Siever, L. J., Buchsbaum, M. S., New, A. S., Spiegel-Cohen, J., Wei, T., Hazlett, E. A., . . . Mitropoulou, V. (1999). D,L-Fenfluramine response in impulsive personality disorder assessed with [18F]fluorodeoxyglu- cose positron emission tomography. Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 413– 423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00111-0

Siever, L. J., Torgersen, S., Gunderson, J. G., Livesley, W. J., & Kendler,

  1. S. (2002). The borderline diagnosis III: Identifying endophenotypes for genetic studies. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 964 –968. http://dx.doi

.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01326-4

Siever, L., & Trestman, R. L. (1993). The serotonin system and aggressive personality disorder. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 33– 3409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004850-199311002-00005

Silva, H., Iturra, P., Solari, A., Villarroel, J., Jerez, S., Vielma, W.,… Roa,

  1. (2007). Serotonin transporter polymorphism and fluoxetine effect on impulsiveness and aggression in borderline personality disorder. Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría, 35, 387–392.

Skodol, A. E. (2012). Personality disorders in DSM-5. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 317–344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev- clinpsy-032511-143131

Skodol, A. E., Siever, L. J., Livesley, W. J., Gunderson, J. G., Pfohl, B., & Widiger, T. A. (2002). The borderline diagnosis II: Biology, genetics, and clinical course. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 951–963. http://dx.doi

.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01325-2

Snyder, S., & Pitts, W. M., Jr. (1984). Electroencephalography of DSM–III borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 69, 129 –134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1984.tb02476.x

Soloff, P. H., Kelly, T. M., Strotmeyer, S. J., Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J. (2003). Impulsivity, gender, and response to fenfluramine challenge in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 119(1–2), 11–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00100-8

Soloff, P. H., Meltzer, C. C., Becker, C., Greer, P. J., & Constantine, D. (2005). Gender differences in a fenfluramine-activated FDG PET study of borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 138, 183–195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.02.008

Soloff, P. H., Meltzer, C. C., Becker, C., Greer, P. J., Kelly, T. M., & Constantine, D. (2003). Impulsivity and prefrontal hypometabolism in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 123, 153–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0925-4927(03)00064-7

Soloff, P. H., Meltzer, C. C., Greer, P. J., Constantine, D., & Kelly, T. M. (2000). A fenfluramine-activated FDG-PET study of borderline person- ality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 540 –547. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00202-4

Soloff, P. H., Price, J. C., Meltzer, C. C., Fabio, A., Frank, G. K., & Kaye,

  1. H. (2007). 5HT2A receptor binding is increased in borderline per- sonality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 580 –587. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.022

Stepp, S. D., Smith, T. D., Morse, J. Q., Hallquist, M. N., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2012). Prospective associations among borderline personality disorder symptoms, interpersonal problems, and aggressive behaviors. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 27, 103–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260511416468

Stoffers, J. M., Völlm, B. A., Rücker, G., Timmer, A., Huband, N., & Lieb, (2012). Psychological therapies for people with borderline personal- ity disorder. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 8(8), CD005652.

Tebartz van Elst, L., Hesslinger, B., Thiel, T., Geiger, E., Haegele, K., Lemieux, L., . . . Ebert, D. (2003). Frontolimbic brain abnormalities in patients with borderline personality disorder: A volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 163–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01743-2

Tedeschi, J. T., & Quigley, B. M. (2000). A further comment on the construct validity of laboratory aggression paradigms: A response to Giancola and Chermack. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5, 127–136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00028-7

Trull, T. J., Solhan, M. B., Tragesser, S. L., Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., Piasecki, T. M., & Watson, D. (2008). Affective instability: Measuring a core feature of borderline personality disorder with ecological momen- tary assessment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 647– 661. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012532

van Langen, M., Wissink, I., van Vugt, E. S., Van der Stouwe, T., & Stams, J. J. M. (2014). The relation between empathy and offending: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19, 179 –189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2014.02.003

Van Voorhees, E. E., Dennis, P. A., Elbogen, E. B., Clancy, C. P., Hertzberg, M. A., Beckham, J. C., & Calhoun, P. S. (2014). Personality Assessment Inventory internalizing and externalizing structure in veter- ans with posttraumatic stress disorder: Associations with aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 582–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.21554

Vogel, F., Wagner, S., Bas¸kaya, O., Leuenberger, B., Mobascher, A., Dahmen, N., . . . Tadic´, A. (2012). Variable number of tandem repeat polymorphisms of the arginine vasopressin receptor 1A gene and im- pulsive aggression in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psy- chiatric Genetics, 22, 105–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/YPG.0b013e32834accad

von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna, I.-A., Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Mundt, C., Fiedler, P., & Resch, F. (2010a). Attentional bias in later stages of emotional information processing in female adolescents with borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 43, 25–32. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1159/000255960

von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna, I.-A., Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Mundt, C., Fiedler, P., & Resch, F. (2010b). Initial orienting to emotional faces in female adolescents with borderline personality disorder. Psychopathol- ogy, 43, 79 – 87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000274176

Wagner, A. W., & Linehan, M. M. (1999). Facial expression recognition ability among women with borderline personality disorder: Implications for emotion regulation? Journal of Personality Disorders, 13, 329 –344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.1999.13.4.329

Wagner, S., Baskaya, O., Anicker, N. J., Dahmen, N., Lieb, K., & Tadic´, (2010). The catechol o-methyltransferase (COMT) val(158)met poly- morphism modulates the association of serious life events (SLE) and impulsive aggression in female patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122, 110 –117. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01501.x

Wagner,  S.,  Baskaya,  O.,  Dahmen,  N.,  Lieb,  K.,  &  Tadic´,  A.  (2010). Modulatory role of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism on the effects of serious life events on impulsive aggres- sion in borderline personality disorder. Genes, Brain & Behavior, 9, 97–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00539.x

Weinstein, Y., Gleason, M. E. J., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2012). Borderline but not antisocial personality disorder symptoms are related to self-reported partner aggression in late middle-age. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 692– 698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028994

Whisman, M. A., & Schonbrun, Y. C. (2009). Social consequences of borderline personality disorder symptoms in a population-based survey: Marital distress, marital violence, and marital disruption. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 410 – 415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.410

Wolf, R. C., Thomann, P. A., Sambataro, F., Vasic, N., Schmid, M., & Wolf, N. D. (2012). Orbitofrontal cortex and impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: An MRI study of baseline brain perfusion. Euro- pean Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 262, 677– 685. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00406-012-0303-1

World Health Organization. (2007). Third Milestones of a Global Cam- paign for Violence Prevention Report 2007: Scaling Up. World report on violence and health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organiza- tion.

Yildirim, B. O., & Derksen, J. J. L. (2012). A review on the relationship between testosterone and life-course persistent antisocial behavior. Psy- chiatry Research, 200, 984 –1010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.07.044

Zetzsche, T., Frodl, T., Preuss, U. W., Schmitt, G., Seifert, D., Leinsinger, G., . . . Meisenzahl, E. M. (2006). Amygdala volume and depressive symptoms in patients with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 302–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.020

Zetzsche, T., Preuss, U. W., Frodl, T., Schmitt, G., Seifert, D., Münch- hausen, E., . . . Meisenzahl, E. M. (2007). Hippocampal volume reduc- tion and history of aggressive behaviour in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 154, 157–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.05.010

Zimmerman, D. J., & Choi-Kain, L. W. (2009). The hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis in borderline personality disorder: A review. Har- vard Review of Psychiatry, 17, 167–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 10673220902996734

Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68, 999 –1029. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00124

Falk Mancke, Sabine C. Herpertz, and Katja Bertsch, Department of General Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg.

This work was supported by two grants of the German Research Foun- dation to Sabine C. Herpertz on borderline personality disorder (Clinical Research Foundation on Mechanisms of Disturbed Emotion Processing in Borderline Personality Disorder, www.kfo256.de; Schmahl et al., 2014: He 2660/12-1; He 2660/7-2).

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Falk Mancke, Department of General Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Voßstraße 2, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany. E-mail: falk.mancke@med.uni- heidelberg.de

 

View publication stats

Comments are closed.